Spatial scaling is an important prerequisite for many spatial tasks and involves an understanding of how distances in different-sized spaces correspond. Previous studies have found evidence for such an understanding in preschoolers; however, the mental processes involved remain unclear. The present study investigated whether children and adults use mental transformations to scale distances in space. Adults and 4-and 5-year-old children (N = 60) were asked to use maps to locate target objects in a larger referent space on a touchscreen. The size of the referent space was held constant, but the sizes of the maps were varied systematically, resulting in seven scaling factors. A linear increase in response times and errors with increasing scaling factor suggested that participants of every age group mentally transformed the size of the map to compare it to the referent, providing evidence for an analog imagery strategy in children's and adults' spatial scaling. Spatial scaling is fundamental to many spatial tasks that require an understanding of how distances in different-sized spaces are related. The ability to map distances from one space to another is involved in many daily activities, such as interpreting navigation aids or imagining the height of a building by looking at its blueprint. Around the age of 3 years, children are able to establish symbolic correspondence between a model and its referent (DeLoache, 1987), but successful mapping between spatial representations also requires an understanding of geometric correspondence (Downs, 1985;Newcombe & Huttenlocher, 2000).A crucial precondition for establishing geometric correspondence is the ability to encode distances in a metric manner. There is evidence that metric coding is present early in life. Looking time studies revealed that 5-month-old infants are sensitive to changes in metric distances (Newcombe, Huttenlocher, & Learmonth, 1999;Newcombe, Sluzenski & Huttenlocher, 2005), and toddlers encode distance metrically in a hide-and-seek game (Huttenlocher, Newcombe, & Sandberg, 1994). Furthermore, magnitude coding is used early in life, as evidenced by infants' discrimination of space, time, number, and speed (Brannon, Lutz, & Cordes, 2006;Brannon, Suanda, & Libertus, 2007;Möhring, Libertus, & Bertin, 2012;Xu & Spelke, 2000), and recent studies yielded evidence for cross-dimensional transfer, suggesting that magnitude information regarding various dimensions is coded in one representational system (de Hevia & Spelke, 2010;Lourenco & Longo, 2010). It is likely that metric understanding is based on this fundamental comparative system, termed the general magnitude system (Walsh, 2003).A second crucial step in establishing geometric correspondence is to map distances from one space to another, which -for different sized spaces -requires spatial scaling.
Running Head: ZOOMING IN ON SPATIAL SCALING 4Previous research has shown that if task demands are low and locations vary on one dimension only, 3-year-olds are able to locate objects in a referent space,...