2002
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000902005172
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The development of regular and irregular verb inflection in Spanish child language

Abstract: We present morphological analyses of verb inflections produced by  Spanish-speaking children (age range :  ;  to  ; ) taken from longitudinal and cross-sectional samples of spontaneous speech and narratives. Our main observation is the existence of a dissociation between regular and irregular processes in the distribution of errors : regular suffixes and unmarked (non-alternating) stems are over-extended to irregulars in children's inflection errors, but not vice versa. We also found that overreg… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
72
0
5

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 122 publications
(84 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
7
72
0
5
Order By: Relevance
“…These differences are suggestive of a U-shaped trend in the development of sigmatic past-tense overapplications in Greek, similarly to what was found for the development of overregularizations in other languages (see e.g. Marcus et al 1992 for English, andClahsen et al 2002 for Spanish). Table 8 shows mean percentages (and standard deviations) of the participants' responses for rhyming novel verbs.…”
Section: Error Analysissupporting
confidence: 71%
“…These differences are suggestive of a U-shaped trend in the development of sigmatic past-tense overapplications in Greek, similarly to what was found for the development of overregularizations in other languages (see e.g. Marcus et al 1992 for English, andClahsen et al 2002 for Spanish). Table 8 shows mean percentages (and standard deviations) of the participants' responses for rhyming novel verbs.…”
Section: Error Analysissupporting
confidence: 71%
“…The dual-route model of inflection (Clahsen, 1999;Clahsen, Aveledo, & Roca, 2002;Clahsen, Rothweiler, & Woest, 1999;Marcus, 1995aMarcus, , 1995bMarcus et al, 1992;Pinker, 1999;Pinker & Prince, 1988;Pinker & Ullman, 2002) proposes that irregularly inflected words (e.g., mice) are stored in an associative memory, whereas the inflection of regular words (e.g., houses) is computed by a default rule (e.g., "add -s" for English plurals) that combines a symbol for a stem with a symbol for a suffix. Before the default rule is acquired, if a child does not have an appropriate inflected form in memory, then she or he will be forced to utter a bare stem in its place (Pinker, 1999), and therefore, errors of omission may occur prior to the acquisition of a rule.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This pattern holds for all other errors of this kind in that there was no single case of a conjugation class error in such cases. For example, errors such as *devolv-a-do, i.e., a regularized form of devuel-t-o with the first conjugation theme vowel -awere nonexistent in the large data set examined by Clahsen et al (2002). These cases indicate that in children's morphological errors the formation of stems can be dissociated from root-related and inflectional processes.…”
Section: Stems and Inflectional Classesmentioning
confidence: 98%