2015
DOI: 10.1080/17586801.2014.1000812
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The developmental pattern of spelling in Catalan from first to fifth school grade

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
27
1
8

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
1
27
1
8
Order By: Relevance
“…It appears, children in the early grades of primary school show capacity to analyse the word morphological, and the identification of the inflectional suffixes, here <-es>, expressing number, in <persones>, and <-er>, expressing the infinitive mode, in <haver>, provides the child with a helpful basis on which to produce the necessary spelling. These results are consistent with previous studies showing that the typological characteristic of the language affect the rate and path of development of an orthographic lexicon (Ravid and Gillis, 2006;Defior et al, 2008;Llaurado and Tolchinsky, 2016).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…It appears, children in the early grades of primary school show capacity to analyse the word morphological, and the identification of the inflectional suffixes, here <-es>, expressing number, in <persones>, and <-er>, expressing the infinitive mode, in <haver>, provides the child with a helpful basis on which to produce the necessary spelling. These results are consistent with previous studies showing that the typological characteristic of the language affect the rate and path of development of an orthographic lexicon (Ravid and Gillis, 2006;Defior et al, 2008;Llaurado and Tolchinsky, 2016).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
“…Ravid and Gillis (2006) showed that young children speaking Hebrew, a language with a highly synthetic morphology, used morphological cues to a greater extent than children of the same age who speak Dutch, a language with a much sparser morphology. A similar effect of a salient morphology was shown for children learning to spell in Spanish (Defior et al, 2009) and Catalan (Llaurado and Tolchinsky, 2016). These studies used a single word spelling task, so whether children use morphological cues to support their text based spelling and improve text quality needs elucidation.…”
Section: Language Typologiesmentioning
confidence: 74%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, /hi:l/ associated with the meaning, "to become healthy, " is spelled <heal>, while associated with the meaning "back part of the human foot" is spelled <heel>. A similar distinction was used in another study, also on Catalan (Llauradó and Tolchinsky, 2016). In short, our adaptation entails that the "orthographic" category in POMAS is subdivided into "orthographic" and "lexical" strategies, where the former are characterized by context-dependent rules and knowledge of legal sequences of letters, whereas the latter require rote memorization of words or parts of words, with or without association to a semantic representation.…”
Section: Adapting Pomas To Assess Catalan Spellingmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Moreover, a majority of studies on the use of non-phonological spelling strategies has been conducted in English or French, both languages with highly inconsistent phonographic mappings (Borgwaldt et al, 2004;Caravolas et al, 2012), which could arguably make such strategies indispensable, in contrast to more consistent orthographies that could rely on phonological representations to a much larger degree. In this study, we examined the use of non-phonological spelling strategies in early-(grade 2) and intermediate-level (grade 4) speakers of Catalan, a Romance language spoken in Barcelona (Spain), with a semi-transparent orthography (Llauradó and Tolchinsky, 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%