2005
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.4.587
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Dynamics of Experimentally Induced Criterion Shifts.

Abstract: Investigations of decision making have typically assumed stationarity, even though commonly observed "context effects" are dynamic by definition. Mirror effects are an important class of context effects that can be explained by changes in participants' decision criteria. When easy and difficult conditions are blocked alternately and a mirror effect is observed, participants must repeatedly change their decision criteria. The authors investigated the time course of these criterion changes and observed the build… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
76
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 63 publications
(77 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
1
76
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In our experiments, 80 trials followed the shift in old item strength. In the studies of Brown and Steyvers (2005) and Benjamin and Bawa (2004), far fewer trials were needed to induce criterion shifts. Still, Experiment 4 can shed light on this question, because any effect of strength on initial criterion placement should be observed within the first 80 test trials.…”
Section: Discussion: Experiments 1-3mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In our experiments, 80 trials followed the shift in old item strength. In the studies of Brown and Steyvers (2005) and Benjamin and Bawa (2004), far fewer trials were needed to induce criterion shifts. Still, Experiment 4 can shed light on this question, because any effect of strength on initial criterion placement should be observed within the first 80 test trials.…”
Section: Discussion: Experiments 1-3mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We attempted to do this while addressing two specific concerns. First, in the context of a lexical decision task, Brown and Steyvers (2005) raised the important point that it takes some time to become aware of changes in test conditions; changes in response bias can lag significantly behind changes in test conditions. In our experiments, 80 trials followed the shift in old item strength.…”
Section: Discussion: Experiments 1-3mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The ability of the hierarchical confidence model to provide the best predictions for some participants, particularly in direct competition with a reduced model that does not include delays, but is otherwise identical-suggests it is an important phenomenon. Delayed sequential effects in decision making are difficult to study and model (e.g., Gao, Wong-Lin, Holmes, Simen, & Cohen, 2009), and the extent of delay is sometimes captured in dynamic decision making models simply by the inclusion of free parameters (Brown & Steyvers, 2005). The hierarchical model, as with the original SRA model on which it is based, provides a viable alternative.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As Speekenbrink and Shanks (2010) point out in their empirical and modeling investigation of cue-based categorization, real-world environments change in many ways. Previous work in modeling human decision making has used environments involving gradual drift (e.g., Otto et al, 2010;Rakow & Miller, 2009), patterns consistent with cyclical change (e.g., Yi, Steyvers, & Lee, 2009), step change jumps (e.g., Brown & Steyvers, 2005), and combinations of all of these sort of dynamics (e.g., Speekenbrink & Shanks, 2010). Our experimental design considered only large discrete changes of two types, encouraging a transition from limited to extensive search, and back again.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation