2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.08.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The early ontogeny of human–dog communication

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

3
160
2
5

Year Published

2008
2008
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 209 publications
(170 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
3
160
2
5
Order By: Relevance
“…In certain situations, dogs' behaviour is probably driven by a motivation to satisfy ostensively signalled human imperatives in the 'here-and-now' (Topál et al 2009b) and the ostensively communicated human action demonstrations can be functionally interpreted as imperatives by dogs with the function of performing the observed action in the presence of (and 'for') the human demonstrator. In agreement with the studies showing specific sensitivity to human's communicative signals in dogs (Erdőhegyi et al 2007;Kaminski et al 2009;Riedel et al 2008), we propose that for the dog, the function of human demonstration is not (only)…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…In certain situations, dogs' behaviour is probably driven by a motivation to satisfy ostensively signalled human imperatives in the 'here-and-now' (Topál et al 2009b) and the ostensively communicated human action demonstrations can be functionally interpreted as imperatives by dogs with the function of performing the observed action in the presence of (and 'for') the human demonstrator. In agreement with the studies showing specific sensitivity to human's communicative signals in dogs (Erdőhegyi et al 2007;Kaminski et al 2009;Riedel et al 2008), we propose that for the dog, the function of human demonstration is not (only)…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…"Momentary distal pointing" provides a diYcult scenario for Wnding hidden food (in comparison to pointing gestures used by Hare et al (2002) or Riedel et al (2007) because (1) the pointing Wnger is more than 50 cm far from the baited bowl, (2) the subjects cannot see the pointing hand during their approach to the bowl, and (3) the handcue is not accompanied by gazing at the bowl. In the case of momentary pointing gestures, the subject has to remember a short and relatively distant signal for some time before making a choice.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…pointing gestures) virtually as well as older dogs that obviously have more human experience. They focused their critique on Riedel et al (2008), who reported in three experiments that puppies as young as 6 weeks old are skilled at using human communicative cues. First, Riedel et al (2008) reported a longitudinal comparison of performance in dogs, aged 6-24 weeks old, with two human pointing cues, a novel marker cue, and a control condition with no cue.…”
Section: Response To Wynne Et Al (2008): Does Human Experience Matter?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically they found that wolves were more skilled than shelter dogs and pet dogs tested outdoors, and equally as skilled as pet dogs tested indoors. In a second study, Wynne et al (2008) critiqued an experiment by Riedel et al (2008) that examined the use of human communicative cues by dog puppies. Riedel and colleagues reported a series of three studies showing that domestic dog puppies comprehended a human pointing gesture as early as 6 weeks of age.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation