2013
DOI: 10.1177/0255761413491210
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of body movement on listeners’ perceptions of musicality in trombone quartet performance

Abstract: The purpose of this study was to determine what effect body movement would have on listeners' (N = 90) perceptions of a professional chamber ensemble performance. Specifically, an audio/video recording of a trombone quartet performance was used for the music stimulus. Listeners were asked to rate each performance on the basis of perceived appropriateness of style and perceived ensemble expressivity. While the video portion of the stimulus changed to reflect each of three movement conditions: (1) deadpan -no ex… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
33
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(42 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
9
33
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, Davidson found a correlation between expressivity ratings and the amount of movement employed-more movement produced higher ratings. This particular finding, concerning the amount of a performers' movement and the correlated ratings of expressivity, has been a common thread in ensuing research (Broughton & Stevens, 2009;Davidson, 1993Davidson, , 1994Davidson, , 1995Juchniewicz, 2008;Sasanfar, 2012;Silveira, 2014;Vines et al, 2011), which suggests that performers should use more movement when desiring to convey heightened perceptions of expression.…”
mentioning
confidence: 75%
“…In addition, Davidson found a correlation between expressivity ratings and the amount of movement employed-more movement produced higher ratings. This particular finding, concerning the amount of a performers' movement and the correlated ratings of expressivity, has been a common thread in ensuing research (Broughton & Stevens, 2009;Davidson, 1993Davidson, , 1994Davidson, , 1995Juchniewicz, 2008;Sasanfar, 2012;Silveira, 2014;Vines et al, 2011), which suggests that performers should use more movement when desiring to convey heightened perceptions of expression.…”
mentioning
confidence: 75%
“…Composite ratings were calculated as the sum of their ratings of the eight performance dimensions, which could range from 8 to 48 with higher scores corresponding to a more favorable performance evaluation. The use of summed scores as an overall evaluation metric has been used in prior research as a supplement to ratings of individual performance dimensions (Morrison, Price, Geiger, & Cornacchio, 2009;Saunders & Holahan, 1997;Silveira, 2014;Springer, 2013), and this practice is commonly used in music performance evaluations (Boyle & Radocy, 1987). Excerpts were labeled by style and applause condition as follows: BH (ballad, high magnitude applause), BL (ballad, low magnitude applause), BN (ballad, no applause), MH (march, high magnitude applause), ML (march, low magnitude applause), and MN (march, no applause).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While still photos were used as a means to control extraneous variables (e.g., using different videos of different ensembles), there was a sacrifice in ecological validity in the design of the study. We recommend that future researchers consider using live recordings of ensembles mimicking performing music while a consistent audio track is applied to the video (similar to Juchniewicz, 2008; Silveira, 2014; VanWeelden & McGee, 2007). Furthermore, while we purposely chose to use Bergee’s (1995) three primary factors given their reliability, validity, and precedence in the research literature, most festival judges are given a performance rubric on which to base their evaluations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%