2013
DOI: 10.1002/acp.2953
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Effect of Different Types of Leading Questions on Adult Eyewitnesses with Mild Intellectual Disabilities

Abstract: Adults with an intellectual disability (ID) are often considered poor witnesses; however, this may depend on the type of questions asked during investigative interviews. We examined the impact of four different types of misleading questions commonly used in interviews. These questions varied in their specificity, presumptive knowledge and structure (open or closed). Forty-one adults with a mild ID watched a short film; they were then interviewed about what had happened. Half of the questions contained misleadi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Research concerning the investigative interviewing of vulnerable witnesses has largely concentrated on children, or on adults with intellectual disability (e.g., Agnew & Powell, 2004; Bowles & Sharman, 2014; Cederborg et al, 2012). This focus has led to broad guidelines that suggest how individuals with communication impairment might be interviewed effectively by investigative interviewers.…”
Section: Investigative Interview Guidelines For Minimally Verbal Adultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research concerning the investigative interviewing of vulnerable witnesses has largely concentrated on children, or on adults with intellectual disability (e.g., Agnew & Powell, 2004; Bowles & Sharman, 2014; Cederborg et al, 2012). This focus has led to broad guidelines that suggest how individuals with communication impairment might be interviewed effectively by investigative interviewers.…”
Section: Investigative Interview Guidelines For Minimally Verbal Adultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Cardone and Dent (1996) compared general and specific questions for adults but did not find any significant impact on suggestibility. In contrast, Bowles and Sharman (2014b) found that the adults with mild ID were most suggestible to misleading information if asked in a ‘presumptive’ (suggestible) style such as a ‘tag’ question, for example, ‘Eric helped himself to a Pepsi, didn’t he?’ A ‘tag’ question is more of a statement, with the question ‘tagged’ on at the end, to invite confirmation. This study was limited in that only one of each question type was asked.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…This poses challenges for cross-examination as the purpose is for the witness to be questioned on the information provided in their examination-in-chief, but a witness with ID may simply agree with alternative suggestions put to them or agree if accused of being mistaken or lying about details of an event. Statements and closed style questions requiring yes/no answers, particularly ‘tag’ questions result in higher levels of suggestibility (Bowles and Sharman, 2014b; Henry and Gudjonsson, 2003, 2007; Perlman et al, 1994). Yet the Kebbell et al (2004) paper showed that closed questions were the most common question type used during cross-examination.…”
Section: Discussion Of Findings and Relevance To Cross-examinationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This sample composition potentially limits the generalizability to other populations; however, we do not believe this to be the case. Research has demonstrated the robustness of the misinformation effect across different samples of younger and older participants, in different countries, with different abilities (e.g., Bowles & Sharman, 2014; Huff & Umanath, 2018; Roebers, Bjorklund, Schneider, & Cassel, 2002; Zhu et al, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%