2000
DOI: 10.1207/s1532690xci1801_02
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Effect of Explaining Another's Actions on Children's Implicit Theories of Balance

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
56
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 72 publications
(60 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
4
56
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For this purpose, we focus the current investigation on children's beliefs about balance relationships. We chose this domain both because children's beliefs about balance have been well established by prior research (e.g., Karmiloff-Smith & Inhelder, 1974;Case, 1985;Halford et al, 2002;Jansen & van der Maas, 2002;McClelland, 1989;McClelland, 1995;Normandeau, Larivee, Roulin, & Longeot, 1989;Shultz & Takane, 2007;Siegler, 1976;Siegler & Chen, 1998;Siegler & Chen, 2002;Pine & Messer, 2000;Raijmakers, van Koten, & Molenaar, 1996) and because balancing blocks are natural and accessible stimuli for exploratory play.…”
Section: Children's Beliefs About Balance Relationshipsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For this purpose, we focus the current investigation on children's beliefs about balance relationships. We chose this domain both because children's beliefs about balance have been well established by prior research (e.g., Karmiloff-Smith & Inhelder, 1974;Case, 1985;Halford et al, 2002;Jansen & van der Maas, 2002;McClelland, 1989;McClelland, 1995;Normandeau, Larivee, Roulin, & Longeot, 1989;Shultz & Takane, 2007;Siegler, 1976;Siegler & Chen, 1998;Siegler & Chen, 2002;Pine & Messer, 2000;Raijmakers, van Koten, & Molenaar, 1996) and because balancing blocks are natural and accessible stimuli for exploratory play.…”
Section: Children's Beliefs About Balance Relationshipsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These studies have typically focused on transitions in children's use of rules and strategies in balance scale tasks (e.g. Case, 1985;Halford, Andrews, Dalton, Boag, & Zielinski, 2002;Jansen & van der Maas, 2002;Normandeau et al, 1989;Siegler, 1976;Siegler & Chen, 1998;Siegler & Chen, 2002;Pine & Messer, 2000;Raijmakers et al, 1996). Consistent with the Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder (1974) study, such research 1 Because we used the Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder (1974) study as a starting point, we adopt their practice of referring to the children's beliefs about balance relations as "theories".…”
Section: Children's Beliefs About Balance Relationshipsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Subsequent studies have shown that prompting for such self-explanations can lead to improved learning outcomes in numerous domains including arithmetic (Calin-Jageman & Ratner, 2005;Rittle-Johnson, 2006;Siegler, 2002), geometry (Aleven & Koedinger, 2002;Wong, Lawson & Keeves, 2003), interest calculations (Renkl, Stark, Gruber & Mandel, 1998), LISP programming (Bielaczyc, Pirolli, & Brown, 1995), argumentation (Schworm & Renkl, 2007), Piagetian number conservation (Siegler, 1995), probability calculation (Große & Renkl, 2003), biology text comprehension (Chi, DeLeeuw, Chiu, & LaVancher, 1994), and balancing beam problems (Pine & Messer, 2000). Moreover, these self-explanation effects have been demonstrated across a wide range of age cohorts, from 5-year-old students (CalinJageman & Ratner, 2005) to adult bank apprentices (Renkl et al, 1998).…”
Section: The Self-explanation Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The vast majority of prior experimental studies have held the number of examples or problems studied constant, with the result that students in the self-explanation conditions spend more time on the intervention (e.g. Atkinson, Renkl & Merrill, 2003;Pine & Messer, 2000;Rittle-Johnson, 2001;Siegler, 1995;Wong, Lawson & Keeves, 2002). Given that generating self-explanations generally requires more time per problem, it may be that self-explanation effects arise simply from encouraging students to spend more time thinking about the material, rather than by some mechanism specific to self-explanation.…”
Section: Explanation Quality As a Predictor Of Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Pine and Messer (2000) found significant improvements when children observed an adult modeling the correct solution and were encouraged to explain what they saw. In general, children in this condition showed greater learning gains than children who simply observed a model but did not produce an explanation.…”
Section: The Representational Redescription Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%