1985
DOI: 10.3758/bf03204914
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of fundamental frequency on the discriminability between pure and tempered fifths and major thirds

Abstract: Thresholds were determined for discrimination between pure and tempered musical intervals (DTs) consisting of simultaneous complex tones. Tempered intervals are characterized by small frequency differences between those harmonics that coincide in pure intervals. Interference of these nearly coinciding harmonics gives rise to the perception of beats. Two parameters of beats were varied independently: (l) beat frequency and (2) depth oflevel variation (temporal envelope) as a measure of DT. DTs were determined f… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

1986
1986
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Numerous psychoacoustic models have been proposed to explain the consonance/dissonance percept [47–51,55,57]. According to these models, the most consonant intervals would be the ones that could be expressed with simple frequency ratios, which has been supported by psychological studies [75,78,79]. Intervals such as the unison (1:1), the octave (2:1), perfect fifth (3:2), and perfect fourth (4:3) are regarded as the most consonant.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Numerous psychoacoustic models have been proposed to explain the consonance/dissonance percept [47–51,55,57]. According to these models, the most consonant intervals would be the ones that could be expressed with simple frequency ratios, which has been supported by psychological studies [75,78,79]. Intervals such as the unison (1:1), the octave (2:1), perfect fifth (3:2), and perfect fourth (4:3) are regarded as the most consonant.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…However, ratings of subjective purity (V os 1985) deviated from precise predictions of the model (discussed earlier) of Plomp & Levelt (1965) and Kameoka & Kuriyagawa (1969a,b), suggesting that modifications are needed. Vos & van Vianen (1985), Hall & Hess (1984), and Elliot et al (1987) found tolerance for mistuning decreased when going from less consonant to more consonant intervals, again implicating absence of beating as a cue for pure tuning. The results of the latter two studies also suggested an additional strategy of matching the intervals to abstract standards of interval sizes.…”
Section: Tuning and Intonationmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Tuning judgments are somewhat more precise for smaller intervals and more consonant intervals (Hall & Hess, 1984). Detection of mistuning can be attributed largely to the strength of beats produced by tempered intervals (Vos, 1982, 1984, 1986, 1988; Vos & van Vianen, 1984, 1985). However, experiments with dichotic stimuli (two tones to different ears that produce no beats) suggest that musicians can also use internal interval standards to make tuning judgments (Hall & Hess, 1984).…”
Section: Pitchmentioning
confidence: 99%