2013
DOI: 10.1515/humor-2013-0011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of joke-origin-induced expectancy on cognitive humor

Abstract: Four experiments explored the effect of humor expectancy on the cognitive evaluation of jokes. Participants read jokes purportedly delivered by celebrity comedians or celebrity non-comedians. Participants reported jokes in the comedian condition to be significantly more amusing (Experiment 1). Furthermore, this effect was repeated in a within-participants replication where celebrity comedians and celebrity non-comedians were matched on positive evaluations (Experiment 2). This indicates that the findings canno… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Laughter and humor are distinctly different phenomena [1,2,4], though the perception of both is influenced socially. The funniness of a joke can be affected by who tells it [9] and by the cultural origins of the joke [10]: in the current study funniness is modulated by the presence and intensity of laughter. Laughter is an extremely salient and important social cue and although laughter can be commonplace, it always carries a wealth of critical social and emotional meaning [2], and we process it even if we are not directed specifically to engage with the laughter [2].…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Laughter and humor are distinctly different phenomena [1,2,4], though the perception of both is influenced socially. The funniness of a joke can be affected by who tells it [9] and by the cultural origins of the joke [10]: in the current study funniness is modulated by the presence and intensity of laughter. Laughter is an extremely salient and important social cue and although laughter can be commonplace, it always carries a wealth of critical social and emotional meaning [2], and we process it even if we are not directed specifically to engage with the laughter [2].…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although researchers have independently studied and demonstrated the effectiveness of cognitive and affective humour on persuasion (Gelb & Zinkhan, 1986; Johnson & Mistry, 2013, Lammers et al., 1983; Unger, 1995), humour research that investigates both cognition and affect simultaneously is called for (Crawford & Gregory, 2015; Vakratsas & Ambler, 1999). Given that consumer behaviour is dynamic and that each decision involves both cognitive and affective processes (Berryessa & Caplan, 2020; Maheswaran & Shavitt, 2000; Punyatoya, 2019; Verhagen & Bloemers, 2018), investigating both aspects is essential to enhance understanding of the effects of ads and improve predictions about consumer responses to humorous ads (Eisend, 2011; Lammers et al., 1983; Vande Velde et al., 2018).…”
Section: Theoretical Framework and Hypotheses Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A non-comedian told the funny stories. People find jokes delivered by comedians to be significantly more amusing (Johnson & Mistry, 2013). When people are told to expect humorous material, the humour effect on memory still emerges (Carlson, 2011).…”
Section: Hypothesismentioning
confidence: 99%