1996
DOI: 10.1080/00221309.1996.9921259
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Effect of Music Amplitude on the Reaction to Unexpected Visual Events

Abstract: The effects of music amplitude on participants' response time to randomly presented, unexpected, visual events were investigated. Ninety participants completed a motor-reaction task without music and with music played at 60, 70, or 80 dBA. Males preferred more intense music than females did, with males selecting a comfort level of 72 dBA and females, 66 dBA. However, participants' reaction time and the total time to respond to a randomly activated red light were independent of gender. All participants responde… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
25
0
3

Year Published

2009
2009
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
1
25
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, music intensity designated as comfortable may be more conducive to superior performance when played concurrently with the task. Turner, Fernandez, and Nelson (1996) asked participants to perform a motor-reaction task in the presence of popular music played at four different intensities (No music, 60 dBA, 70 dBA, and 80 dBA). Participants performed significantly faster in the 70 dBA condition, wherein the music intensity was closest to their comfort levels.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, music intensity designated as comfortable may be more conducive to superior performance when played concurrently with the task. Turner, Fernandez, and Nelson (1996) asked participants to perform a motor-reaction task in the presence of popular music played at four different intensities (No music, 60 dBA, 70 dBA, and 80 dBA). Participants performed significantly faster in the 70 dBA condition, wherein the music intensity was closest to their comfort levels.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, if the driving task is not demanding, a secondary task may not affect task performance, and in some conditions, may enhance arousal and enhance driving performance. This is in keeping with both the Yerkes-Dodson curve and with Easterbrook's hypothesis that too much or too little arousal will lead to poorer allocation of attentional resources (de Waard, 1996;Solovey, Zec, Perez, Reimer, & Mehler, 2014;Turner, Fernandez, & Nelson, 1996;Ünal, de Waard, Epstude, Steg, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 51%
“…An SPER digital sound level meter (model no. 840028), held 70-80 cm from the speakers, was used to measure sound volume and keep it near or below 70 dB, which is close to most listeners' comfortable level (Turner, Fernandez, & Nelson, 1996). Music was played by a pair of speakers positioned to the left and right of the computer monitor, positioned approximately 70-80 cm from the participant.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Los artículos recabados ascendieron a un total de 46 (Anexos 1 y 2). Con respecto al tipo de estudios, la distribución fue la siguiente: 42 estudios experimentales [1,2,6,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,37,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,49,51,50,52], un estudio de revisión [48], uno descriptivo [38], un estudio exploratorio [7] y un meta-análisis [26]. La productividad anual de las publicaciones registradas permitió hallar nueve publicaciones en el año 2012; seis publicaciones en 2013; cuatro publicaciones en el año 2019; tres publicaciones en los años 2007 y 2018; dos publicaciones en los años 1999, 2002, 2003, 2009, 2010, 2014, y 2016; y finalmente se halló una publicación en cada uno de los años…”
Section: Características De Los Estudiosunclassified