2019
DOI: 10.1177/1362168819883894
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of vocal production on vocabulary learning in a second language

Abstract: The Production Effect (PE) is a memory phenomenon, referring to memory advantage for produced items (read aloud) over non-produced items (read silently). Since vocalizing shows consistent memory benefits, it has been suggested as a mnemonic that can be used to assist vocabulary learning. The present study investigated the PE in L2 vocabulary learning and examined whether learning is durable over time. Hebrew speaking students learned new words in Esperanto, either by vocal production or by no-production. Reten… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

2
17
2

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
2
17
2
Order By: Relevance
“…At study, a larger number of unique processes are involved while encoding the produced items relative to the nonproduced items. The additional unique encoding information leads to enhanced memory [22, 23]. This rationale well explains the superiority of vocalizing visually presented text over silent reading, and the advantage of writing aurally presented material over listening.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…At study, a larger number of unique processes are involved while encoding the produced items relative to the nonproduced items. The additional unique encoding information leads to enhanced memory [22, 23]. This rationale well explains the superiority of vocalizing visually presented text over silent reading, and the advantage of writing aurally presented material over listening.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The production effect has been shown to persist across a wide variety of stimulus types, including nonwords ([18], Expt. 6) or unfamiliar (L2) words [22], pictures [23], and text [24]. This latter finding of Ozubko and his colleagues [24] is of special importance as the study involved more complex material (sentences and essays), and the tests were educationally relevant (fill-in-the-blank, FITB tests).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While the production effect is strongest when adults generate the words themselves, it is also reported when an experimenter or another participant generates the word (MacLeod, 2011). The production effect is seen under different training conditions such as repeating or recalling novel words (Kan, Sadagopan, Janich, & Andrade, 2014;Krishnan, Watkins, & Bishop, 2017), and it also persists after delayed testing (Icht & Mama, 2019;Kaushanskaya & Yoo, 2011;Ozubko, Hourihan, & MacLeod, 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
“…It is the processing of that same word on different levels that contributes to stronger encoding, for example, when reading aloud a word, which involves auditory and articulatory processing, versus when reading silently, which involves only visual processing. This has the effect of creating a more robust record in memory, and further retrieval is facilitated (e.g., Icht & Mama, 2019;Jones & Pyc, 2014;Mama & Icht, 2016;Ozubko et al, 2014;Pritchard et al, 2019;Zamuner, Morin-Lessard, Strahm, & Page, 2016).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Additionally, it can be speculated that a further factor which might have supported target vocabulary learning and retention in the OR+SW condition was the vocal production of target words during the performance of the OR task. The positive effect of vocal production of target words on L2 vocabulary development was confirmed by Icht and Mama (2019). The vocal production of target words might have formed a phonological representation of new L2 words in learners' memory which probably supported the acquisition of the target words.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 73%