2020
DOI: 10.1037/xap0000258
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effectiveness of refutation texts to correct misconceptions among educators.

Abstract: Existing evidence indicates that teachers endorse a considerable number of misconceptions about education and neuroscience that hinder the adoption of evidence-based practices in the classroom. The present study found that refutation texts can be an effective means to correct educational misconceptions, although their effects were only temporary and did not change teachers' intention to adopt educational practices based on those erroneous ideas.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
19
1
3

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
(109 reference statements)
0
19
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Most studies include a follow-up (Step 4) that consists in administering the same neuromyth survey as in Step 1 and 3. Ferrero et al (2020a) first measured endorsement of 18 neuromyths and 18 neuro-hit survey statements (presented in random order) in a sample of in-service teachers (Step 1). Fortyfive days later, participants read refutation texts (Step 2).…”
Section: Are Refutation-based Interventions Effective At Dispelling Neuromyths and Are Corrective Effects Enduring In Time?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most studies include a follow-up (Step 4) that consists in administering the same neuromyth survey as in Step 1 and 3. Ferrero et al (2020a) first measured endorsement of 18 neuromyths and 18 neuro-hit survey statements (presented in random order) in a sample of in-service teachers (Step 1). Fortyfive days later, participants read refutation texts (Step 2).…”
Section: Are Refutation-based Interventions Effective At Dispelling Neuromyths and Are Corrective Effects Enduring In Time?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Results typically show that a direct retraction significantly reduces reliance on the critical information relative to the no-retraction control condition, but does not eliminate the influence down to the no-misinformation baseline (e.g., Ecker, Hogan, & Lewandowsky, 2017;Ecker, Lewandowsky, & Apai, 2011). Continued influence has also been demonstrated with real-world news (Lewandowsky, Stritzke, Oberauer, & Morales, 2005), common myths (Ferrero, Hardwicke, Konstantinidis, & Vadillo, 2020;Sinclair, Stanley, & Seli, 2019;Swire, Ecker, & Lewandowsky, 2017), political misconceptions (Ecker & Ang, 2019; also see Nyhan & Reifler, 2010;Wood & Porter, 2019), with subtle and implicit misinformation (Ecker, Lewandowsky, Chang, & Pillai, 2014;Rich & Zaragoza, 2016), false allegations (Thorson, 2016; but see Ecker & Rodricks, 2020), and when the misinformation is presented initially as a negation that is later reinstated .…”
Section: Continued Influence Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Results typically show that a direct retraction significantly reduces reliance on the critical information relative to the no-retraction control condition, but does not eliminate the influence down to the no-misinformation baseline (e.g., E c k e r , H o g a n , & L e w a n d o w s k y , 2 0 1 7 ; Ecker, Lewandowsky, & Apai, 2011). Continued influence has also been demonstrated with real-world news (Lewandowsky, Stritzke, Oberauer, & Morales, 2005), common myths (Ferrero, Hardwicke, Konstantinidis, & Vadillo, 2020;Sinclair, Stanley, & Seli, 2019;Swire, Ecker, & Lewandowsky, 2017), political misconceptions (Ecker & Ang, 2019; also see Ecker, Sze, & Andreotta, 2021;Nyhan & Reifler, 2010;Wood & Porter, 2019), with subtle and implicit misinformation (Ecker, Lewandowsky, Chang, & Pillai, 2014;Rich & Zaragoza, 2016), false allegations (Thorson, 2016; but see Ecker & Rodricks, 2020), and when the misinformation is presented initially as a negation that is later reinstated .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%