1991
DOI: 10.1016/0747-5632(91)90018-v
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effects of human versus computer authorship on consumers' perceptions of psychological reports

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
4
0

Year Published

1996
1996
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
2
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…They found that highly experienced users may be overly skeptical and that inexperienced users tend to over‐rely on suggestions from computers, as compared to a control condition with mimeographed lists of the same suggestions. This finding is consistent with an early belief among researchers that users are likely to be “in awe” of computers, viewing them as credible in a wide range of domains (Pancer, George, & Gebotys, 1992); however, subsequent experimental research has shown little evidence for this belief (e.g., Andrews & Gutkin, 1991; Wærn & Ramberg, 1996).…”
Section: Multidimensional Approaches To Credibilitysupporting
confidence: 84%
“…They found that highly experienced users may be overly skeptical and that inexperienced users tend to over‐rely on suggestions from computers, as compared to a control condition with mimeographed lists of the same suggestions. This finding is consistent with an early belief among researchers that users are likely to be “in awe” of computers, viewing them as credible in a wide range of domains (Pancer, George, & Gebotys, 1992); however, subsequent experimental research has shown little evidence for this belief (e.g., Andrews & Gutkin, 1991; Wærn & Ramberg, 1996).…”
Section: Multidimensional Approaches To Credibilitysupporting
confidence: 84%
“…Andrews and Gutkin () examined educators’ perceptions of computer‐generated versus psychologist‐written psychological reports and found no difference in quality or credibility by authorship. They also failed to find a difference in diagnostic impressions and confidence in findings by authorship.…”
Section: Included Studies’ Results Discussion and Research Recommenmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The use of technical jargon in psychological reports has been discussed and investigated for some time and this review found teachers’ to mostly favor and better understand reports with less technical jargon. While most research found reports written with less jargon to be rated as more useful and comprehensible (Wiese et al, ), some research found no effect or reports with heavy jargon to be rated as more useful, professional, and understandable (Andrews & Gutkin, ; Fletcher et al, ; Raforth & Richmond, ). One study found teachers were dissatisfied with the high degree of technical jargon used in psychological reports (Rahill, ).…”
Section: Included Studies’ Results Discussion and Research Recommenmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…They found that highly experienced users may be overly skeptical and that inexperienced users tend to over-rely on suggestions from computers, as compared to a control condition with mimeographed lists of the same suggestions. This finding fits with an early belief among researchers that users are likely to be "in awe" of computers, viewing them as credible in a wide range of domains (Pancer, 1992), but later experiments show little evidence for this belief (Andrews and Gutkin, 1991;Waern and Ramberg, 1996 (Singletary, 1976) and (Vandenbergh, Soley, and Reid, 1981) asked participants in their studies to imagine a specific high-credibility source (in Singletary's case, a news person; in VandenBergh et al's, an advertiser) and to list as many terms as possible that, in the participant's view, gave credibility to that source. Other researchers either sampled the existing literature to create a list of candidate terms based upon their review or relied upon intuition.…”
Section: Contexts Affect Credibility Constructssupporting
confidence: 74%