2013
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-013-0323-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effects of list composition and perceptual fluency on judgments of learning (JOLs)

Abstract: The perceptual fluency hypothesis proposes that items that are easier to perceive at study will be given higher memorability ratings, as compared with less fluent items. However, prior research has examined this metamemorial cue primarily using mixed-list designs. Furthermore, certain memory effects are moderated by the design (mixed list vs. pure list) used to present stimuli. The present study utilized mixed as well as pure lists to assess whether judgments of learning based on perceptual fluency are relativ… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

10
65
2

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 63 publications
(77 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
10
65
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Although some studies suggested that some revision or additional assumptions may be required (e.g., Jang & Nelson, 2005;Kimball & Metcalfe, 2003), most findings were consistent with its predictions (e.g., Castel, 2008;Fraundorf & Benjamin, 2014;Koriat, Bjork, Sheffer, & Bar, 2004;Kornell, Rhodes, Castel, & Tauber, 2011;Soderstrom & McCabe, 2011). Most important for present purposes, a growing body of evidence suggests that mnemonic cues do indeed affect JOLs (e.g., Benjamin, Bjork, & Schwartz, 1998;Castel, McCabe, & Roediger, 2007;Koriat & Ma'ayan, 2005;Matvey, Dunlosky, & Guttentag, 2001;Susser et al, 2013;Undorf & Erdfelder, 2011, 2013. There is also some evidence that beliefs about memory may influence JOLs (for a review, see Bjork, Dunlosky, & Kornell, 2013).…”
supporting
confidence: 65%
“…Although some studies suggested that some revision or additional assumptions may be required (e.g., Jang & Nelson, 2005;Kimball & Metcalfe, 2003), most findings were consistent with its predictions (e.g., Castel, 2008;Fraundorf & Benjamin, 2014;Koriat, Bjork, Sheffer, & Bar, 2004;Kornell, Rhodes, Castel, & Tauber, 2011;Soderstrom & McCabe, 2011). Most important for present purposes, a growing body of evidence suggests that mnemonic cues do indeed affect JOLs (e.g., Benjamin, Bjork, & Schwartz, 1998;Castel, McCabe, & Roediger, 2007;Koriat & Ma'ayan, 2005;Matvey, Dunlosky, & Guttentag, 2001;Susser et al, 2013;Undorf & Erdfelder, 2011, 2013. There is also some evidence that beliefs about memory may influence JOLs (for a review, see Bjork, Dunlosky, & Kornell, 2013).…”
supporting
confidence: 65%
“…More importantly, the font size exerted an influence on JOLs even when other diagnostic cues (e.g., relatedness of word pairs) were available, suggesting that people use perceptual cues to guide their monitoring. This font-size effect has been further confirmed by subsequent studies (e.g., Kornell, Rhodes, Castel, & Tauber, 2011;Miele, Finn, & Molden, 2011;Mueller, Dunlosky, Tauber, & Rhodes, 2014;Susser, Mulligan & Besken, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…Importantly, all of experiments conducted by Rhodes and Castel (2008) and by Mueller et al (2014) used a within-subjects manipulation of font size, which therefore directly contrasted the large-font words with the small-font words. Susser, Mulligan, and Besken (2013) demonstrated that a between-subjects manipulation eliminated the font size effect in JOLs (see Yue, Castel, & Bjork, 2013, for a similar finding with word clarity). We would predict that any similar metacognitive illusion will also be eliminated in a study context that fails to directly contrast the two encoding tasks or conditions.…”
Section: Metacognitive Illusionsmentioning
confidence: 60%