2014
DOI: 10.1037/a0035031
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effects of numerical magnitude, size, and color saturation on perceived interval duration.

Abstract: The relative magnitude (or intensity) of an event can have direct implications on timing estimation. Previous studies have found that greater magnitude stimuli are often reported as longer in duration than lesser magnitudes, including Arabic digits (Xuan, Zhang, He, & Chen, 2007). One explanation for these findings is that different quantitative dimensions (size, intensity, number) are processed and represented according to a common analog magnitude system (Walsh, 2003). In the current study, we examined wheth… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
18
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 69 publications
2
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, the magnitudes of task-irrelevant symbolic numbers impact judgments about physical size and length (De Hevia, Girelli, Bricolo, & Vallar, 2008;A Henik & Tzelgov, 1982;Viarouge & de Hevia, 2013), numerosity (Naparstek & Henik, 2010), duration (AlardsTomalin, Leboe-McGowan, Shaw, & Leboe-McGowan, 2014;Kiesel & Vierck, 2009;Oliveri et al, 2008;Vicario et al, 2008;Xuan, Chen, He, & Zhang, 2009;Xuan, Zhang, He, & Chen, 2007), and luminance (i.e., brightness) (Cohen Kadosh et al, 2008;Cohen Kadosh, Henik, & Walsh, 2007; but see Pinel et al, 2004). Furthermore, they also interfere with basic spatial-motor response selection, including the performance speed of left vs. right handed responses (Dehaene, Bossini, & Giraux, 1993;Nuerk, Wood, & Willmes, 2005), precision motor responses (pinch vs. whole hand grasps) responses, and grip aperture (Andres, Ostry, Nicol, & Paus, 2008;Lindemann, Abolafia, Girardi, & Bekkering, 2007).…”
Section: Generalized Magnitude Systemmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…For example, the magnitudes of task-irrelevant symbolic numbers impact judgments about physical size and length (De Hevia, Girelli, Bricolo, & Vallar, 2008;A Henik & Tzelgov, 1982;Viarouge & de Hevia, 2013), numerosity (Naparstek & Henik, 2010), duration (AlardsTomalin, Leboe-McGowan, Shaw, & Leboe-McGowan, 2014;Kiesel & Vierck, 2009;Oliveri et al, 2008;Vicario et al, 2008;Xuan, Chen, He, & Zhang, 2009;Xuan, Zhang, He, & Chen, 2007), and luminance (i.e., brightness) (Cohen Kadosh et al, 2008;Cohen Kadosh, Henik, & Walsh, 2007; but see Pinel et al, 2004). Furthermore, they also interfere with basic spatial-motor response selection, including the performance speed of left vs. right handed responses (Dehaene, Bossini, & Giraux, 1993;Nuerk, Wood, & Willmes, 2005), precision motor responses (pinch vs. whole hand grasps) responses, and grip aperture (Andres, Ostry, Nicol, & Paus, 2008;Lindemann, Abolafia, Girardi, & Bekkering, 2007).…”
Section: Generalized Magnitude Systemmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…These findings are consistent with previous studies reporting many interference effects of various non-temporal magnitude dimensions on concurrent temporal processing. For example, a duration was judged longer subjectively if it was larger in size (Xuan et al, 2007; Alards-Tomalin et al, 2014; Rammsayer and Verner, 2014), consisted of more items (Mo and Michalski, 1972; Long and Beaton, 1981; Dormal et al, 2006; Xuan et al, 2007; Hayashi et al, 2013), had a higher intensity (Berglund et al, 1969; Ekman et al, 1969; Goldstone et al, 1978; Matthews et al, 2011), had a higher speed (Kanai et al, 2006), contained stimuli with a longer line (Casasanto and Boroditsky, 2008), presented in the right hemispace (Vicario et al, 2008), or occupied a body gesture covering longer spatial distance (Cai et al, 2013). Traditionally, those effects were explained in a theoretic framework of commonly shared representational system among various magnitude dimensions, including time, space and quantity.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has been well-established that temporal processing 1 is influenced by temporal as well as non-temporal information (e.g., Fortin et al, 1993; Brown, 1997; Mattes and Ulrich, 1998; Oliveri et al, 2008; Eagleman and Pariyadath, 2009). Specifically, various non-temporal, magnitude dimensions, such as stimulus size, number of contained items, intensity and speed, exert critical impact on perceived durations (Berglund et al, 1969; Mo and Michalski, 1972; Dormal et al, 2006; Kanai and Watanabe, 2006; Xuan et al, 2007; Alards-Tomalin et al, 2014; Rammsayer and Verner, 2014). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are two lines of research that, taken together, suggest that there will be a sample size bias on estimates of average duration. The first consists of studies showing that estimates of duration can be biased by other nontemporal dimensions of a stimulus (e.g., Alards-Tomalin, Leboe-McGowan, Shaw, & Leboe-McGowan, 2014;Brigner, 1986;Fabbri, Cancellieri, & Natale, 2012;Matthews, Stewart, & Wearden, 2011;Xuan, Zhang, He, & Chen, 2007; for a review, see Matthews & Meck, 2016). For example, Casasanto and Boroditsky (2008) had participants estimate the amount of time that a line was displayed onscreen and found that duration estimates tended to increase as the length of the line increased.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%