2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.10.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effects of perceptual similarity and category membership on early word-referent identification

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
96
0
2

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 65 publications
(103 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
5
96
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Regarding early semantics, Arias-Trejo and Plunkett (18) find that both visual similarity and category membership contribute to semantic competition: For toddlers, understanding "shoe" in the context of a boot and a shoe was harder than when shoe appeared with a hat or bin instead. Thus, even in seasoned word learners, certain visual contexts make it harder to ascertain a spoken word's referent.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Regarding early semantics, Arias-Trejo and Plunkett (18) find that both visual similarity and category membership contribute to semantic competition: For toddlers, understanding "shoe" in the context of a boot and a shoe was harder than when shoe appeared with a hat or bin instead. Thus, even in seasoned word learners, certain visual contexts make it harder to ascertain a spoken word's referent.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Young children are also sensitive to the similarity between spoken referents and pictured objects (e.g., Arias-Trejo & Plunkett, 2010;Styles & Plunkett, 2009;Torkildsen et al, 2007; see also Mani & Plunkett, 2010). Recent studies have demonstrated that language-mediated shifts of attention to only partially matching referents in the visual field are even present in children as young as 3 years of age.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Depending on the age and/or vocabulary size of the participants, it is common practice to adjust the onset of the post-naming phase to account for variation in gaze shift latency (Fernald et al, 2008). In addition, many factors can influence the processing time of the target and distracter pictures, starting with the nature and complexity of the auditory stimulus, the visual properties of the stimuli (e.g., Arias-Trejo & Plunkett, 2010), or the type of distracter (familiar vs. unfamiliar; White & Morgan, 2008). Therefore a time window fixed a priori may not be the most accurate.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This time window usually ends at 2000 ms, as it is generally considered that later looking behaviour is no longer related to the processing of the auditory stimulus. When plots of the time course for proportions of looks to the target are included, usually at end of the result section (Arias-Trejo & Plunkett, 2010;Fernald et al, 2008;Swingley & Aslin, 2000), the time window can then be justified a posteriori, the visual inspection confirming that roughly 2000 ms after word onset, looking behaviour resumes to chance level (that is, equal looks to the target and the distracter). However, since latency is a function of at least age (Zangl & Fernald, 2007) and vocabulary size (Fernald et al, 2006), the whole looking behaviour can also be influenced by task difficulty.…”
Section: Methods Of Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation