2013
DOI: 10.1167/13.9.524
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Effects of Target Typicality on Guidance and Verification in Categorical Search

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One explanation might be attributable to a limitation on visual working memory: If only a finite number of feature values can be encoded into the target template (e.g., Olivers, Peters, Houtkamp, & Roelfsema, 2011), and hue information is more useful than other feature dimensions, orientation and shape may be selectively excluded from this limited set of guiding features. Another possibility is that precise visual details from a target preview may be relied upon less commonly than what has been believed; this possibility was suggested by recent work using a categorical search task (Maxfield, Stadler, & Zelinsky, 2013; Schmidt & Zelinsky, 2009; Yang & Zelinsky, 2009; Yu, Maxfield, & Zelinsky, 2016). If participants construct a categorical representation of the target to guide their search, when previewed with a red sports car they might search for any red sports car rather than the specific car shown at preview.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One explanation might be attributable to a limitation on visual working memory: If only a finite number of feature values can be encoded into the target template (e.g., Olivers, Peters, Houtkamp, & Roelfsema, 2011), and hue information is more useful than other feature dimensions, orientation and shape may be selectively excluded from this limited set of guiding features. Another possibility is that precise visual details from a target preview may be relied upon less commonly than what has been believed; this possibility was suggested by recent work using a categorical search task (Maxfield, Stadler, & Zelinsky, 2013; Schmidt & Zelinsky, 2009; Yang & Zelinsky, 2009; Yu, Maxfield, & Zelinsky, 2016). If participants construct a categorical representation of the target to guide their search, when previewed with a red sports car they might search for any red sports car rather than the specific car shown at preview.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…All images were closely cropped using a rectangular marquee to depict only the object and a minimal amount of background. Because object typicality can affect categorization and search (Murphy & Brownell, 1985, Maxfield et al, 2014), targets were selected to be typical members of their category at the subordinate, basic, and superordinate levels. We did this by having 45 participants complete a preliminary norming task in which 240 images (5 exemplars from each of 48 subordinate categories) were rated for both typicality and image agreement (Snodgrass & Vanderwart, 1980) at each hierarchical level using a 1 (high typicality/image agreement) to 7 (low typicality/image agreement) scale.…”
Section: Behavioral Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We evaluate the visual representation of common object categories using a categorical search task (Maxfield, Stadler, & Zelinsky, 2014; Schmidt & Zelinsky, 2009; Zelinsky, Adeli, Peng, Samaras, 2013; Zelinsky, Peng, Berg, & Samaras, 2013; Zelinsky, Peng, & Samaras, 2013). Categorical search differs from standard visual search in that targets are designated by category (e.g., the word “dog”) instead of by a picture pre-cue (e.g., an image of a specific dog), a situation that rarely exists outside the laboratory.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…WM is necessary for all visual detection task, where a predefined target object is kept online, to be detected at a later stage in a search display that may or may not be presented among other (distractor) objects (e.g., Olivers, Meijer, & Theeuwes, 2006; Soto, Heinke, Humphreys, & Blanco, 2005). The detection can be less efficient when the target and distractor objects share one or more features (Cowan, 1988; Duncan & Humphreys, 1989; Maxfield, Stadler, & Zelinsky, 2013). In any visual search, a template of the cue or target object is activated, and the search is based on the match between the cue template and the actual target.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%