2020
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2020.0090
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The evolution of high-fidelity social learning

Abstract: A defining feature of human culture is that knowledge and technology continually improve over time. Such cumulative cultural evolution (CCE) probably depends far more heavily on how reliably information is preserved than on how efficiently it is refined. Therefore, one possible reason that CCE appears diminished or absent in other species is that it requires accurate but specialized forms of social learning at which humans are uniquely adept. Here, we develop a Bayesian model to contrast the evolution of high-… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
13
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
1
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, the fact that the in-group-copying bias produced some amount of cultural divergence in both of our experiments is of particular interest from a cultural evolutionary point of view. This is because the exceptional complexity of human culture and technology (Montrey & Shultz, 2020) likely depends on integrating and recombining diverse cultural traits (Mesoudi & Thornton, 2018). Theory suggests that for cultural evolution to be cumulative (i.e., for complexity to increase over time), populations may have to be fragmented to some degree so that unique traits have the opportunity to flourish (Derex et al, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Finally, the fact that the in-group-copying bias produced some amount of cultural divergence in both of our experiments is of particular interest from a cultural evolutionary point of view. This is because the exceptional complexity of human culture and technology (Montrey & Shultz, 2020) likely depends on integrating and recombining diverse cultural traits (Mesoudi & Thornton, 2018). Theory suggests that for cultural evolution to be cumulative (i.e., for complexity to increase over time), populations may have to be fragmented to some degree so that unique traits have the opportunity to flourish (Derex et al, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As humans, we rely heavily on social learning, which allows us to benefit from others’ knowledge without incurring the same costs or undertaking the same risks (Montrey & Shultz, 2020). However, copying is not always adaptive (Kendal et al, 2018).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Humans are an exceptionally social species for whom cultural transmission and social learning is rampant and essential (see Boyd et al., 2011; Call et al., 2007; Jiménez & Mesoudi, 2019; Montrey & Shultz, 2020). Yet, as even Henrich and Gil‐White (2001) note, social learning is not unique to humans (as reviewed in Cacchione & Amici, 2020; Heyes & Galef, 1996).…”
Section: Why Might We Expect To Observe Prestige‐based Hierarchies In...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…2 Formal theories and models of cultural evolution must be able to accommodate discontinuities, because they permeate all branches of culture, including art, science, and technology, as well as economic and political systems (Kuhn, 1962;Wilson, 1973;Bar-Yosef, 1998). They present a formidable challenge for models of cultural evolution, which often have built-in assumptions about how new innovations build on established knowledge (Lewis and Laland, 2012;Montrey and Shultz, 2020). The assumption that new knowledge in a domain builds incrementally on existing knowledge in that domain is inconsistent with findings that entrenchment in established practices and perspectives can hinder innovation, and cultural breakthroughs often come about by striking out in an altogether new direction (Frensch and Sternberg, 1989;Wiley, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%