1996
DOI: 10.1007/bf00144024
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The experience of spatiality for congenitally blind people: A phenomenological-psychological study

Abstract: This phenomenological-psychological study aims at discovering the essential constituents involved in congenitally blind people's spatial experiences. Nine congenitally blind persons took part in this study. The data were made up of half structured (thorough) interviews. The analysis of the data yielded the following three comprehension forms of spatiality; (i) Comprehension in terms of image-experience; (ii) Comprehension in terms of notions; (iii) Comprehension in terms of knowledge.Comprehension in terms of … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

1999
1999
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is perhaps helpful to compare the experiential situation with that of blind persons, who cannot see material objects approaching (before being already in touch with them), or with that of traumatized individuals, who are equally unable to foresee when or where a horror-triggering experience presents itself (until it already does). In both cases, objects seem to emerge out of nowhere and recede back into this “engulfing ‘nothingness”’ (see Tustin, 1990 , 218; Karlsson, 1996 ). 9 Regardless of the obvious differences between these cases, and regardless of questions of severity, the comparison is helpful, insofar as autistic individuals, too, tend to remain particularly alert and on guard vis-à-vis the potentially impinging surroundings.…”
Section: The Collapse Of Intentional Depthmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is perhaps helpful to compare the experiential situation with that of blind persons, who cannot see material objects approaching (before being already in touch with them), or with that of traumatized individuals, who are equally unable to foresee when or where a horror-triggering experience presents itself (until it already does). In both cases, objects seem to emerge out of nowhere and recede back into this “engulfing ‘nothingness”’ (see Tustin, 1990 , 218; Karlsson, 1996 ). 9 Regardless of the obvious differences between these cases, and regardless of questions of severity, the comparison is helpful, insofar as autistic individuals, too, tend to remain particularly alert and on guard vis-à-vis the potentially impinging surroundings.…”
Section: The Collapse Of Intentional Depthmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These traits may consist of prejudices, attitudes, notions, thoughts and feelings that give form to the way in which people experience a phenomenon. Karlsson (1996) emphasises that a phenomenological analysis is not a mere phenomenal description because it aims to describe 'the logos of the phenomenon, that is to say those necessary constituents (structure) which are needed in order for just that particular phenomenon to be what it is ' (p. 307). The researcher's role is to collect as concrete a set of descriptions as possible, reflect upon those descriptions and carry out an interpretative analysis (Hellström et al 1999;Karlsson and Sjöberg 2009;Leiviskä et al 2011).…”
Section: Phenomenology and Empirical Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(That is not even to mention the information we can obtain from the sound produced by our shoes.) According to Gunnar Karlsson (1996), the haptic sense can be considered as the most important source of information for blind people.…”
Section: This Change In Attention Is Beautifully Illustrated In Leuvementioning
confidence: 99%