2005
DOI: 10.1007/s11166-005-1153-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Gambler’s Fallacy and the Hot Hand: Empirical Data from Casinos

Abstract: Research on decision making under uncertainty demonstrates that intuitive ideas of randomness depart systematically from the laws of chance. Two such departures involving random sequences of events have been documented in the laboratory, the gambler’s fallacy and the hot hand. This study presents results from the field, using videotapes of patrons gambling in a casino, to examine the existence and extent of these biases in naturalistic settings. We find small but significant biases in our population, consisten… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

13
213
0
7

Year Published

2009
2009
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 314 publications
(233 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
13
213
0
7
Order By: Relevance
“…Individuals adhering to the gambler's fallacy (GF) appear to assume non-independence between sequential outcomes, consistent with the belief that the probability of obtaining a reward decreases after recent reinforcement; consequently, they increase their probability of choosing an action that was not recently rewarded (Estes, 1964;Tversky and Kahneman, 1971). Individuals in a real casino have demonstrated the fallacy (Croson and Sundali, 2005), and when non-humans (e.g., a computer or slot machine) generate sequences of events, the sequences are considered more likely to show the negative recency expected by GF (Ayton and Fischer, 2004). More recent work has examined factors modulating GF and the extent to which it represents rationality (Hahn and Warren, 2009;Barron and Leider, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…Individuals adhering to the gambler's fallacy (GF) appear to assume non-independence between sequential outcomes, consistent with the belief that the probability of obtaining a reward decreases after recent reinforcement; consequently, they increase their probability of choosing an action that was not recently rewarded (Estes, 1964;Tversky and Kahneman, 1971). Individuals in a real casino have demonstrated the fallacy (Croson and Sundali, 2005), and when non-humans (e.g., a computer or slot machine) generate sequences of events, the sequences are considered more likely to show the negative recency expected by GF (Ayton and Fischer, 2004). More recent work has examined factors modulating GF and the extent to which it represents rationality (Hahn and Warren, 2009;Barron and Leider, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…This can be illustrated by the gambler's fallacy: After one has observed a streak of five times black at the roulette table, it is very hard to avoid the feeling that ''it is time for red'' now, which may be a sufficient motivation to bet on it (Croson and Sundali 2005). However, there exist important individual differences with regard to how strongly people are prone to that misperception, and with regard to how much they give into that misperception and bet on it (Scheibehenne and Studer 2014;Scheibehenne et al 2011).…”
Section: Perceiving Illusory Patterns and Betting On Themmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Terrell (1994) investigates field data from horse races and finds that betting behavior is consistent with the gambler's fallacy. Croson and Sundali (2005) investigate the betting behavior of roulette players in a casino in Reno, Nevada. They find that a long streak of the same outcome leads players to bet disproportionately on the opposite outcome.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%