A meta-analysis of the available judgment and memory data on the sleeper effect in persuasion is presented. According to this effect, when people receive a communication associated with a discounting cue, such as a noncredible source, they are less persuaded immediately after exposure than they are later in time. Findings from this meta-analysis indicate that recipients of discounting cues were more persuaded over time when the message arguments and the cue had a strong initial impact. In addition, the increase in persuasion was stronger when recipients of discounting cues had higher ability or motivation to think about the message and received the discounting cue after the message. These results are discussed in light of classic and contemporary models of attitudes and persuasion.Persuasive messages are often accompanied by information that induces suspicions of invalidity. For instance, recipients of communications about a political candidate may discount a message coming from a representative of the opponent party because they do not perceive the source of the message as credible (e.g., Lariscy & Tinkham, 1999). Because the source of the political message serves as a discounting cue and temporarily decreases the impact of the message, recipients may not be persuaded by the advocacy immediately after they receive the communication. Over time, however, recipients of an otherwise influential message may recall the message but not the noncredible source and thus become more persuaded by the message at that time than they were immediately following the communication. The term sleeper effect has been used to denote such a delayed increase in persuasion observed when the discounting cue (e.g., noncredible source) becomes unavailable or "dissociated" from the communication in the memory of the message recipients (Hovland, Lumsdaine, & Sheffield, 1949). Because the sleeper effect concerns initial message impact, as well as recall of the information presented in the communication, the phenomenon has implications for broad models of persuasion, including early learning approaches (e.g., Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953) as well as more recent conceptualizations, such as the heuristic-systematic model and the elaborationlikelihood model (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986;.The sleeper effect is counterintuitive because, generally, the impact of a persuasive communication is greater when one measures the effect closer to the presentation rather than farther away from the time of reception Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). As such, this phenomenon has stimulated a large amount of research about the possibility of increased persuasion over time, as well as the potential decrease and lack of longitudinal change in persuasion (for reviews, see Cook, Gruder, Hennigan, & Flay, 1979;Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). Moreover, if one can understand the conditions that elicit increases, decreases, and stability in persuasion, one should be able to explain the Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.Correspondence concerning this art...