2016
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000915000835
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The impact of input quality on early sign development in native and non-native language learners

Abstract: This is the accepted version of the paper.This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Permanent repository link:

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
22
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
2

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 89 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
0
22
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…While many parents continue to speak to their deaf infant after the early diagnosis of deafness and up to the point of the CI procedure, profound deafness is a major barrier to the uptake and storage of spoken phonology during this early sensitive period. Although parents sometimes use a signed language during this early period, the input they provide is typically at a basic level because they usually begin to learn a signed language only after their child is diagnosed with a hearing impairment (Lu, Jones, & Morgan, 2016). Moreover, despite advancing CI technology, these devices provide a reduced version of the auditory signal so that, once hearing is restored, input, although improved, may remain impoverished to some degree.…”
Section: Delayed Exposure To Language With or Without Prior Exposurementioning
confidence: 99%
“…While many parents continue to speak to their deaf infant after the early diagnosis of deafness and up to the point of the CI procedure, profound deafness is a major barrier to the uptake and storage of spoken phonology during this early sensitive period. Although parents sometimes use a signed language during this early period, the input they provide is typically at a basic level because they usually begin to learn a signed language only after their child is diagnosed with a hearing impairment (Lu, Jones, & Morgan, 2016). Moreover, despite advancing CI technology, these devices provide a reduced version of the auditory signal so that, once hearing is restored, input, although improved, may remain impoverished to some degree.…”
Section: Delayed Exposure To Language With or Without Prior Exposurementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The vast majority of deaf children who are exposed to signing, however, begin to learn sign language from hearing parents. Although these hearing parents choose to expose their deaf child to a sign language, they are themselves adult late learners of sign with limited fluency (Mitchell & Karchmer, 2004;Lu, Jones & Morgan, 2016); the children are also exposed to spoken language, although access to speech is limited because of the child's deafness. These deaf children of hearing parents thus develop a form of bilingualism across both spoken and signed languages (Woll, 2013), but their acquisition of each is likely delayed when compared both to native signing peers (deaf or hearing) acquiring a signed language and hearing speaking peers acquiring a spoken language.…”
Section: Vocabulary Development and Gesture In Deaf Learners Of Sign mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the vast majority of deaf children—approximately 95%—are born to hearing parents who do not sign (Mitchell and Karchmer ) and so they do not usually have access to sign language, at least during the early stages of language acquisition (Lu et al . ). Deaf children as a group are therefore at high risk of language delays.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Signed languages provide a more easily accessible language input, and for the small proportion of deaf children who are born to deaf signing parents ('native signers') signed language development can proceed with very similar milestones and timescale to spoken language acquisition in hearing children (Anderson and Reilly 2002, Mayberry and Squires 2006, Newport and Meier 1985. However, the vast majority of deaf children-approximately 95%are born to hearing parents who do not sign (Mitchell and Karchmer 2004) and so they do not usually have access to sign language, at least during the early stages of language acquisition (Lu et al 2016). Deaf children as a group are therefore at high risk of language delays.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%