2003
DOI: 10.3758/bf03194850
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The influence of contrast and spatial factors in the perceived shape of boundaries

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
21
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
0
21
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One important factor that contributes to the perception of transparent layers is edge contrast polarity (Adelson, 2000; Roncato and Casco, 2003). But transparent layering cannot be explained solely by sorting layers based on contrast polarity.…”
Section: Comparison With Gamut Relativity Theory: Transparency and “Dmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One important factor that contributes to the perception of transparent layers is edge contrast polarity (Adelson, 2000; Roncato and Casco, 2003). But transparent layering cannot be explained solely by sorting layers based on contrast polarity.…”
Section: Comparison With Gamut Relativity Theory: Transparency and “Dmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This illusion is not novel on the phenomenal level, but instead we have a new explanation to apply, based on the notion of local "association field" illustrated in Figure 1, defined as a set of local orientation signals that radiate by contour extrapolation in all directions (Field, Hayes, & Hess, 1993;Shipley & Kellman, 2003). Inspired by this model we have studied the spatial conditions by which local oriented edges are combined into a unique spatial contour using a yes-no detection paradigm (Roncato & Casco, 2003, 2006, and found the precise values of the spatial parameters (relative separation and/or alignment) that allow neighboring edge segments to bind into a smooth contour, in contrast with the values that degrade the perception and cause the edges to no longer conform to a smooth contour. Our previous results show that binding occurs between the association field projections propagating from the two edges, and that these edges are perceived as a unique contour with illusory tilt, providing that two conditions are met: first, the projections have to have the same contrast polarity and, second, their vertical and horizontal distances have to be short: their vertical misalignment (D2) has to be smaller than 7 arcmin and the horizontal gap (D1) has to be smaller than 13 arcmin.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For these effects—both binding and induction—to arise it not necessary that four surfaces converge at the same point as seen in the image of an X-junction. The edges can be non-collinear and their endings can be separated by a gap; in fact if the distance and the deviation of colinearity are within a short range (Roncato and Casco, 2003 , 2006 ) the effects are clearly visible. This explains why we perceive induction effects even with configurations where the X-junctions are not replicated (Bressan, 2001 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Gabor units sharing the same CP are easier to perceptually group when distributed among other Gabor units (McIlhagga and Mullen, 1996 ; Field et al, 2000 ); real or illusory contours are easier to detect when of the same CP (Cavanagh and Leclerc, 1989 ; Spehar, 2000 ). Research on orientation misperception (Roncato and Casco, 2003 , 2006 ; Van Lier and Csathó, 2006 ) demonstrated that this tendency manifests locally as illusory tilts at the edge extremities when they have the same contrast polarity and extend within a spatial range of 7–15°.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%