2011
DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.1878498
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Innocence Effect

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
22
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
0
22
1
Order By: Relevance
“…2;Zimmerman & Hunter, 2018). Moreover, innocent and guilty participants may arrive at different expectations for conviction on the basis of available information (Bibas, 2004;Daftary-Kapur, Zottoli, & Faust, 2019;Gazal-Ayal & Tor, 2012). Thus, examining discount and PTS under different probabilities of conviction (either provided to or estimated by participants) is an important avenue for future research.…”
Section: Limitations and Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…2;Zimmerman & Hunter, 2018). Moreover, innocent and guilty participants may arrive at different expectations for conviction on the basis of available information (Bibas, 2004;Daftary-Kapur, Zottoli, & Faust, 2019;Gazal-Ayal & Tor, 2012). Thus, examining discount and PTS under different probabilities of conviction (either provided to or estimated by participants) is an important avenue for future research.…”
Section: Limitations and Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…1; Tor, Gazal‐Ayal, & Garcia, ). While innocent mock defendants are substantially less likely to plead guilty – even when it may be in their best interest to do so (a phenomenon that has been dubbed the Innocence Effect ; Gazal‐Ayal & Tor, ) – false guilty pleas in the laboratory have been observed to increase when the plea sentence is sharply discounted from the potential trial sentence (Bordens, ) and as potential trial sentence becomes more severe relative to the plea sentence (Zimmerman & Hunter, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Gazal-Ayal and Tor (2012) have argued that, ceteris paribus, innocent defendants are more likely to reject plea bargains compared to guilty defendants. Explanations for this "innocence effect" center on the fact that innocent defendants may: (1) reach legitimately different assessments of trial outcomes due to private information; (2) be irrationally optimistic about their prospects (i.e., underestimate θ); and/or (3) maintain their innocence as a matter of principle (Gross 2011;Tor et al 2010).…”
Section: Innocence and Defendants' Perception Of Judgment Errormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our method can also be used to inform recent attempts to assess the "shadow trial" model (Abrams 2011;Bushway and Redlich 2012;Ulmer and Bradley 2006) and the "innocence effect" (Gazal-Ayal and Tor 2012). The shadow trial approach seeks to model the plea bargain decision as a rational one based on the perceived probable outcome of the trial.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%