2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.07.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The lexical bias effect is modulated by context, but the standard monitoring account doesn’t fly: Related beply to Baars et al. (1975)

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

19
105
0
2

Year Published

2006
2006
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 69 publications
(126 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
19
105
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The impact of lexical information on phoneme processing appears to be modulated by the degree of attention to lexical information ( [52][53][54] and similar effects have been found on speech production [55] and reading [56]). Norris et al [4] argued that to account for this attentional modulation, interactive models would have to turn off feedback, thereby making them autonomous.…”
Section: Attentional Modulation Of Lexical Effectsmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…The impact of lexical information on phoneme processing appears to be modulated by the degree of attention to lexical information ( [52][53][54] and similar effects have been found on speech production [55] and reading [56]). Norris et al [4] argued that to account for this attentional modulation, interactive models would have to turn off feedback, thereby making them autonomous.…”
Section: Attentional Modulation Of Lexical Effectsmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Previous research has shown that speakers are more likely to produce errors resulting in new words than pseudowords, indicating that lexicality is a filter used to intercept errors during speech production (Baars, Motley, & MacKay, 1975;Hartsuiker, Corley & Martensen, 2005;Nooteboom, 2005). Returning to our task, the following three predictions can be made: 1) Impaired language processing of any sort (including monitoring) would be apparent through slower response times and/or higher error rates; 2) Impaired self-monitoring for accuracy would be indexed by an overall increased error-rate; 3) Impaired selfmonitoring for lexicality would result in a modulation of errors as a function of their lexical status.…”
Section: The Current Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is not clear exactly what elements of speech are monitored, when this monitoring occurs, and how production reacts to such monitoring. Furthermore, although the inner monitor has been proposed to be sensitive to all sorts of possible errors, it seems that speakers do not always monitor all of these dimensions, and many theories propose that the editor can be more or less sensitive depending on attentional state (e.g., Oomen & Postma, 2002;Postma, 1997) or strategic factors (Baars et al, 1975;Hartsuiker, Corley, & Martensen, 2005). Together, this makes it very difficult to generate specific predictions or to evaluate post hoc explanations, since the monitor is potentially, but not necessarily, sensitive to everything that can be comprehended.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is worth noting, however, that an inner monitor is only required to explain the mixed error effect (and the lexical bias effect) in models of speech production that prohibit feedback (e.g., Levelt et al, 1999). In theories that allow feedback from phonological levels to lexical levels (e.g., Dell, 1986;Dell & Reich, 1981;Rapp & Goldrick, 2000) these effects can be explained without relying on an inner monitor (see Humphreys, 2002, for evidence that the lexical bias effect is better explained by feedback than by monitoring, but see Hartsuiker et al, 2005, for evidence of strategic effects on lexical bias). Thus, a critical assumption of the monitor-based explanation of the mixed error effect (and of monitor-based explanations of error detection in general) is that the monitor is less able to detect erroneous outcomes that are more similar to the intended utterance than outcomes that are less similar.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%