2017
DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000311
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The magic of words reconsidered: Investigating the automaticity of reading color-neutral words in the Stroop task.

Abstract: In 2 variants of the color-word Stroop task, we compared 5 types of color-neutral distractors-real words (e.g., HAT), pseudowords (e.g., HIX), consonant strings (e.g., HDK), symbol strings (e.g., #$%), and a row of Xs (e.g., XXX)-as well as incongruent color words (e.g., GREEN displayed in red). When participants named the color, relative to a row of Xs, words and pseudowords interfered equally and more than the consonant strings, which in turn interfered more than the symbols. In contrast, when participants i… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

14
83
3
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(101 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
14
83
3
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Importantly, as Rayner and Raney (1996) noted, the absence of an influence of word frequency on eye movement behaviour during target word search does not necessarily imply that lexical processing does not occur for non-target words during search for a word in text. For fixated words at least, lexical access may still occur automatically (MacLeod, 1991; but see Kiefer & Martens, 2010;Kinoshita, De Wit, & Norris, 2017) but, as described in the previous paragraph, it may only rarely modulate control of eye movements. It could be that other methods, such as Event Related…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Importantly, as Rayner and Raney (1996) noted, the absence of an influence of word frequency on eye movement behaviour during target word search does not necessarily imply that lexical processing does not occur for non-target words during search for a word in text. For fixated words at least, lexical access may still occur automatically (MacLeod, 1991; but see Kiefer & Martens, 2010;Kinoshita, De Wit, & Norris, 2017) but, as described in the previous paragraph, it may only rarely modulate control of eye movements. It could be that other methods, such as Event Related…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…responding vocally by saying the colour name aloud) determines the magnitude and type of facilitation and interference that results (Fennell & Ratcliff, 2019;Glaser & Glaser, 1989;Kinoshita, De Wit & Norris, 2017;McClain, 1983;Redding & Gerjets, 1977;Sharma & McKenna, 1998;Turken & Swick, 1999;Zahedi, Abdel Rahman, Stürmer & Sommer, 2019; see also MacLeod, 1991;Parris, Hasshim, Wadsley, Augustinova, & Ferrand, submitted). For example, Sharma and McKenna (1998) reported evidence they argued showed no semantic Stroop effects with the manual response Stroop task (although see Brown & Besner, 2001).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Sharma and McKenna (1998) reported evidence they argued showed no semantic Stroop effects with the manual response Stroop task (although see Brown & Besner, 2001). Accounts of differences between these two response modes propose differential access to the systems (i.e., phonological, lexico-semantic, or response level processing) that are assumed to produce interference and facilitation (Glaser & Glaser, 1989;Kinoshita, De Wit & Norris, 2017;Sharma & McKenna, 1998;Sugg & McDonald, 1994;Turken and Swick, 1999;Virzi & Egeth, 1985; see also Fennell & Ratcliff, 2019;Zahedi et al, 2019). Consistent with this, it has recently been argued that vocal and manual responding involve different tasks (naming vs classification, respectively) and as such the type of evidence that is accumulated during Phonological processing in the Stroop task 4 Stroop task performance is different, leading to qualitatively different Stroop effects (Kinoshita et al, 2017).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Evidence showing that this is clearly a possibility comes from studies looking at unintentional word reading, by means of non-colour word Stroop tasks (for a review, see Kinoshita, De Wit, & Norris, 2017). These studies demonstrate that the extent to which the orthographic make-up of a word (i.e., its sublexical letter sequences) invites reading aloud is a prime determinant of colour classification latencies: these are slower for letter strings that are most prototypical of the language (e.g., Klein, 1964;Monsell, Taylor, & Murphy, 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%