2013
DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2012.705861
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The mechanism underlying lexical selection: Evidence from the picture–picture interference paradigm

Abstract: In two experiments using the picture–picture and picture–word interference paradigms, we compared predictions from the swinging lexical network and the response exclusion hypothesis to determine whether the process of word selection is competitive. Further, we suggest that previous categorical effects in the picture–picture interference paradigm were due to stimuli confounds, thus readdressing the debate concerning categorical effects in the paradigm. Consistent with both hypotheses, in Experiment 1 we found f… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The observations of accumulative (i.e., within block) and extended (i.e., over many repetitions) semantic facilitation in the blocked naming paradigm, together with our proposed account of the experimental conditions under which interference arises, collectively undermine the view of lexical selection by competition (e.g., Abdel Rahman & Melinger, 2007, 2009a, 2011; Aristei et al, 2011; Belke, 2008; Belke & Stielow, in press; Belke et al, 2005a; 2005b; Costa, Miozzo, Caramazza, 1999; Damian & Bowers, 2003; Damian et al, 2001; Geng, Kirchgessner, & Schnur, 2013; Hantsch et al, 2005, 2009; 2012; Humphreys, Lloyd-Jones, & Fias, 1995; La Heij, 1988; Levelt et al, 1999; Piai et al, 2012; Roelofs, 1992, 2003; Roelofs & Piai, in press; Santesteban et al, 2006; Schriefers et al, 1990; Starreveld & La Heij, 1995; Vigliocco et al, 2004). As discussed above, there have been a number of ways in which the hypothesis of lexical competition has been squared with problematic findings in the picture-word paradigm (Piai et al, 2012; Roelofs & Piai, in press; Abdel Rahman & Melinger, 2009a; Hantsch & Madebach, 2013; but see Mahon & Caramazza, 2009; Mahon & Navarrete, under review; Navarrete & Mahon, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…The observations of accumulative (i.e., within block) and extended (i.e., over many repetitions) semantic facilitation in the blocked naming paradigm, together with our proposed account of the experimental conditions under which interference arises, collectively undermine the view of lexical selection by competition (e.g., Abdel Rahman & Melinger, 2007, 2009a, 2011; Aristei et al, 2011; Belke, 2008; Belke & Stielow, in press; Belke et al, 2005a; 2005b; Costa, Miozzo, Caramazza, 1999; Damian & Bowers, 2003; Damian et al, 2001; Geng, Kirchgessner, & Schnur, 2013; Hantsch et al, 2005, 2009; 2012; Humphreys, Lloyd-Jones, & Fias, 1995; La Heij, 1988; Levelt et al, 1999; Piai et al, 2012; Roelofs, 1992, 2003; Roelofs & Piai, in press; Santesteban et al, 2006; Schriefers et al, 1990; Starreveld & La Heij, 1995; Vigliocco et al, 2004). As discussed above, there have been a number of ways in which the hypothesis of lexical competition has been squared with problematic findings in the picture-word paradigm (Piai et al, 2012; Roelofs & Piai, in press; Abdel Rahman & Melinger, 2009a; Hantsch & Madebach, 2013; but see Mahon & Caramazza, 2009; Mahon & Navarrete, under review; Navarrete & Mahon, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…100 ms with distractor words (Glaser & Düngelhoff, 1984) and with distractor pictures (Jescheniak et al, 2009). Similarly, semantic interference in picture naming rarely exceeds 30 to 40 ms with distractor words (e.g., Alario et al, 2000; Damian & Martin, 1999; Geng et al, 2013; Lupker, 1979; Schriefers et al, 1990; cf. Glaser & Düngelhoff, 1984).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A core assumption of this non-competitive account is that semantic interference is restricted to distractor words. A number of studies using distractor pictures instead of distractor words (picture-picture interference task) have produced mixed results, with some studies showing semantic facilitation or no effect (e.g., La Heij, Heikoop, Akerboom, & Bloem, 2003;Damian & Bowers, 2003;Geng, Kirchgessner, & Schnur, 2013) and others showing semantic interference (e.g., Jescheniak, Matushanskaya, Mädebach, & Müller, 2014;Matushanskaya, Mädebach, Müller, & Jescheniak, 2016; see also Aristei, Zwitserlood, & Abdel Rahman, 2012). A recent comprehensive series of picture-picture interference experiments from our lab, using different kinds of materials, stimulus configurations, and task cues, however, has showed semantic interference from distractor pictures quite consistently (Mädebach, Wöhner, & Jescheniak, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…100 ms with distractor words (Glaser & Düngelhoff, 1984) and with distractor pictures (Jescheniak et al, 2009). Similarly, semantic interference in picture naming rarely exceeds 30 -40 ms with distractor words (e.g., Alario, Segui & Ferrand, 2000;Damian & Martin, 1999;Geng et al, 2013;Lupker, 1979;Schriefers et al, 1990;cf. Glaser & Düngelhoff, 1984).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%