2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.08.009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The metacognitive abilities of children and adults

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
13
2
2

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
1
13
2
2
Order By: Relevance
“…This interpretation is supported by comments made by the children as they selected additional objects (e.g., "I think I know, but let me ask just one more question, to be sure"). Although these results are surprising in light of previous research showing that children of this age tend to be overconfident (e.g., Finn & Metcalfe, 2014;Salles, Semelman, Sigman, & Calero, 2016), they are consistent with research on children's decision making, which finds that younger children tend to be more exhaustive in their search than older children (Davidson, 1991a(Davidson, , 1991b. Moreover, Legare and colleagues (2013) found that the preschoolers (4to 6-year-olds) who asked more confirmatory questions (i.e., redundant based on the information already gathered) performed better overall in a 20-questions game, suggesting that the confirmatory strategy, although inefficient, may be useful.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 75%
“…This interpretation is supported by comments made by the children as they selected additional objects (e.g., "I think I know, but let me ask just one more question, to be sure"). Although these results are surprising in light of previous research showing that children of this age tend to be overconfident (e.g., Finn & Metcalfe, 2014;Salles, Semelman, Sigman, & Calero, 2016), they are consistent with research on children's decision making, which finds that younger children tend to be more exhaustive in their search than older children (Davidson, 1991a(Davidson, , 1991b. Moreover, Legare and colleagues (2013) found that the preschoolers (4to 6-year-olds) who asked more confirmatory questions (i.e., redundant based on the information already gathered) performed better overall in a 20-questions game, suggesting that the confirmatory strategy, although inefficient, may be useful.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 75%
“…The task used in this study was adapted from a classic non-verbal paradigm used to measure monitoring abilities in humans (adults and children) 34 as well as non-human primates 35 . The task comprised 114 trials, each starting with a perceptual judgment followed by a report of confidence in performance.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While preschoolers and first graders have strong difficulties in using feedback at all (Lipko, Dunlosky, & Merriman, ), older elementary school students seem to start implementing feedback (Labuhn et al, ). However, even when presented with feedback, children in the later elementary school years still appear to be at least partially deficient in detecting and correcting errors (Hacker, ; Lipko‐Speed et al, ; Salles, Ais, Semelman, Sigman, & Calero, ).…”
Section: Children's Use Of Feedbackmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although children as well as adults are sensitive to the wishful thinking bias, this bias is much stronger in children than in adults (Bjorklund & Green, ). Therefore, it may be more difficult for children to learn from feedback on errors as they may remain overly optimistic even after receiving negative feedback (Destan, Spiess, de Bruin, van Loon, & Roebers, ; Salles et al, ).…”
Section: Children's Use Of Feedbackmentioning
confidence: 99%