2015
DOI: 10.1037/xge0000080
|View full text |Cite|
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The mind in the object—Psychological valuation of materialized human expression.

Abstract: [Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported in Vol 144(4) of Journal of Experimental Psychology: General (see record 2015-33206-002). In the article the labels on the X-axis of Figure 1 "Remove Variance" and "Preserve Variance" should be switched.] Symbolic material objects such as art or certain artifacts (e.g., fine pottery, jewelry) share one common element: The combination of generating an expression, and the materialization of this expression in the object. This explains why people place … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
18
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
0
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Of all species, only modern homo sapiens has evolved the ability to use complex symbolic behavior to organize and coordinate large anonymous societies [1,2,3].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Of all species, only modern homo sapiens has evolved the ability to use complex symbolic behavior to organize and coordinate large anonymous societies [1,2,3].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In psychological and consumer research, the perceived value of artifacts has been measured in diverse ways, including general value (high/low value in isolation, or high/low value relative to a duplicate; for example, Kreuzbauer et al, 2015 ), monetary value (estimated price, willingness to pay, lottery simulations; for example, Fuchs et al, 2015 ; Job et al, 2017 ; Newman & Bloom, 2012 ; Smith et al, 2016 ), perceived quality (high/low overall quality; for example, Job et al, 2017 ), personal value (whether the person likes the object, or wants to buy, consume, touch, own, or show the object to others; for example, Job et al, 2017 ; Stavrova et al, 2016 ), distress at the prospect of the object being destroyed (e.g., Job et al, 2017 ), and perceived value to society (whether the object should be kept in a museum, put in a time capsule, or receive an award; for example, Valsesia et al, 2015 ). The effect of creation history on artifact evaluation is likely to depend on the kind of evaluation.…”
Section: The Western Folk Theory Of Artifact Creationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The lay concept of art has received the most attention from researchers in social and cognitive psychology, encompassing several other psychological phenomena, such as intention-based contagion ( Stavrova et al, 2016 ), the valuation of materialized human expression ( Kreuzbauer et al, 2015 ), the valuation placed on individual effort in art ( Smith & Newman, 2014 ), viewing artworks as part of the “extended self” ( Newman & Bloom, 2012 ; Newman et al, 2014 ), the valuation of creative control ( Valsesia et al, 2015 ), and temporal contagion ( Smith et al, 2016 ). There is evidence of both mental and physical property transmission in art; observers see the visual form of artwork as representing the internal mental states of the artist and also see the individual artifact as containing physical properties transferred from the artist (e.g., the “individual essence” of the artist; Newman et al, 2014 ).…”
Section: Lay Concepts Of Art/craft and Industrial Productionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations