2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.09.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The neural basis of the speed–accuracy tradeoff

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

37
638
12
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 666 publications
(688 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
37
638
12
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This shift to striatum-mediated learning has also been found when participants performed the weather prediction task under cognitive load (Foerde et al, 2006). In studies of the speed-accuracy tradeoff, increased activation was found in the striatum during trials with speed emphasis compared to trials with accuracy emphasis (Forstmann et al, 2008;van Veen et al, 2008), suggesting that enhanced striatal activity may be critical in reducing inhibitory control and facilitating speeded responses (Bogacz et al, 2010). Here, we significantly expand these findings by directly demonstrating that increased time pressure decreases the involvement of frontal and parietal regions and shifts probabilistic multi-cue decision-making to the striatum, even after the completion of initial cue learning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 64%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This shift to striatum-mediated learning has also been found when participants performed the weather prediction task under cognitive load (Foerde et al, 2006). In studies of the speed-accuracy tradeoff, increased activation was found in the striatum during trials with speed emphasis compared to trials with accuracy emphasis (Forstmann et al, 2008;van Veen et al, 2008), suggesting that enhanced striatal activity may be critical in reducing inhibitory control and facilitating speeded responses (Bogacz et al, 2010). Here, we significantly expand these findings by directly demonstrating that increased time pressure decreases the involvement of frontal and parietal regions and shifts probabilistic multi-cue decision-making to the striatum, even after the completion of initial cue learning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 64%
“…Specifically, learning the weather prediction task under stress induced by the cold pressor test has been associated with increased use of implicit, striatum-mediated strategies (Schwabe and Wolf, 2012). Similarly, time pressure on perceptual decision-making has been associated with a deterioration in information processing in early sensory areas (Ho et al, 2012) and increased activity in the striatum (Bogacz et al, 2010;Forstmann et al, 2008), indicating that the striatum may promote faster but possibly premature or sub-optimal decisions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The advantage of model-based analysis of neural data is strongly enhanced by using a dynamic model. Thus far, such analyses have been limited to models that, although they may have a dynamic component, only specify the internal state of the participant at the level of a whole trial (Bogacz, Wagenmakers, Forstmann, & Nieuwenhuis, 2010;Turner et al, 2013). A dynamic model like the one we have presented here makes predictions about the entire trajectory of internal states through which a participant is expected to pass on the way toward making a final recognition decision.…”
Section: Prospects For Neurosciencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…This speed-accuracy tradeoff can be straightforwardly implemented in sequential sampling models, which not only account for performance in a wide range of perceptual tasks (Brown & Heathcote, 2005;Ratcliff & McKoon, 2008) but can also be plausibly linked to neurophysiological correlates of decision making (Bogacz, Wagenmakers, Forstmann, & Nieuwenhuis, 2010;Forstmann et al, 2008Forstmann et al, , 2010Forstmann et al, , 2011Ho et al, 2012;Ivanoff, Branning, & Marois, 2008;Philiastides, Ratcliff, & Sajda, 2006;van Veen, Krug, & Carter, 2008). In general, decision making in sequential sampling models is based on the accumulation of evidence over time until a boundary (or criterion) is reached and an associated response is initiated (Brown & Heathcote, 2005Busemeyer & Townsend, 1993;Diederich & Busemeyer, 2006;Hübner, Steinhauser, & Lehle, 2010;Ratcliff & Smith, 2004;Ratcliff, 1978;Usher & McClelland, 2001;White, Ratcliff, & Starns, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%