2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.08.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The neurophysiological basis of the integration of written and heard syllables in dyslexic adults

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

4
35
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
4
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Typical readers showed an enhancement of the MMN in response to deviant spoken syllables compared with standards when letters were presented with the speech stimuli (Froyen, Willems, & Blomert, 2011) but not when scrambled images were presented with the speech (Mittag, Thesleff, Laasonen, & Kujala, 2013). This enhancement of the MMN was absent in children with dyslexia (Froyen et al, 2011) and adults (Mittag et al, 2013). The lack of enhancement of the MMN in people with dyslexia suggests that early and automatic letter-speech sound integration is absent (Froyen et al, 2011;Mittag et al, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 78%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Typical readers showed an enhancement of the MMN in response to deviant spoken syllables compared with standards when letters were presented with the speech stimuli (Froyen, Willems, & Blomert, 2011) but not when scrambled images were presented with the speech (Mittag, Thesleff, Laasonen, & Kujala, 2013). This enhancement of the MMN was absent in children with dyslexia (Froyen et al, 2011) and adults (Mittag et al, 2013). The lack of enhancement of the MMN in people with dyslexia suggests that early and automatic letter-speech sound integration is absent (Froyen et al, 2011;Mittag et al, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 78%
“…The MMN is evoked in an oddball paradigm when, in a sequence of auditory stimuli, a rarely presented sound (the deviant) deviates from a frequently presented sound (the standard). Typical readers showed an enhancement of the MMN in response to deviant spoken syllables compared with standards when letters were presented with the speech stimuli (Froyen, Willems, & Blomert, 2011) but not when scrambled images were presented with the speech (Mittag, Thesleff, Laasonen, & Kujala, 2013). This enhancement of the MMN was absent in children with dyslexia (Froyen et al, 2011) and adults (Mittag et al, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 87%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This phoneme–grapheme binding becomes automatic for most readers by second grade, after sufficient practice and experience (Blomert, 2011; Froyen, Willems, & Blomert, 2011), resulting in a stable integrated “graphoneme” object representation (Whitney & Cornelissen, 2005). However, for some individuals, this process remains difficult, reflected in lower reading ability Poorer readers have been demonstrated to be able to learn phoneme–grapheme pairings similarly to typical readers but do not automatically draw on this information even after years of instruction and practice (Froyen et al, 2011, p. 644; Mittag, Thesleff, Laasonen, & Kujala, 2013). Thus, this integration of auditory phonological and visual orthographic linguistic information is critical for reading acquisition, and individual differences in this skill may directly impact reading ability.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As mentioned before, reading impairment might reflect a deficit in audiovisual processing and, indeed, children and adult dyslexic readers have been shown to inadequately process audiovisual objects, for instance, while being presented with audiovisual and unisensory letters and speech sounds (Blau, van Atteveldt, Ekkebus, Goebel, & Blomert, 2009;Blau et al, 2010;Froyen, Willems, & Blomert, 2011;Kast, Bezzola, Jäncke, & Meyer, 2011;Kronschnabel, Brem, Maurer, & Brandeis, 2014;Mittag, Thesleff, Laasonen, & Kujala, 2013), while identifying unisensory and audiovisual speech (e.g., Hayes, Tiippana, Nicol, Sams, & Kraus, 2003), and while matching non-linguistic audiovisual materials (e.g., rectangles and tones, Widmann, Schröger, Tervaniemi, Pakarinen, & Kujala, 2012). For a subset of the sample of participants tested in this study, we have recently shown differences between dyslexic and typical adult readers in their audiovisual temporal sensitivity (Francisco, Jesse, Groen, & McQueen, 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%