2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.01.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The neuropsychology of fear learning and fear regulation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

20
218
6
4

Year Published

2005
2005
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 249 publications
(248 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
20
218
6
4
Order By: Relevance
“…In conclusion, the current data clearly revealed a dissociation between fear learning as indexed by conditioned startle potentiation and the learning of stimulus relationships as indexed by conditioned SCR discrimination, thus supporting a two-level account of human fear conditioning (Hamm and Weike, 2005;Ö hman and Mineka, 2001). In a typical human fear conditioning experiment, the subjects not only learn to fear the CS (emotional learning) (LeDoux, 1996) but they also learn on a cognitive level that one CS predicts the occurrence of the UCS [contingency learning (Rescorla, 1988); propositional learning (Lovibond and Shanks, 2002)].…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 76%
“…In conclusion, the current data clearly revealed a dissociation between fear learning as indexed by conditioned startle potentiation and the learning of stimulus relationships as indexed by conditioned SCR discrimination, thus supporting a two-level account of human fear conditioning (Hamm and Weike, 2005;Ö hman and Mineka, 2001). In a typical human fear conditioning experiment, the subjects not only learn to fear the CS (emotional learning) (LeDoux, 1996) but they also learn on a cognitive level that one CS predicts the occurrence of the UCS [contingency learning (Rescorla, 1988); propositional learning (Lovibond and Shanks, 2002)].…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 76%
“…Furthermore, the enhanced SCL to G-CTX in the remote-test group might be explained by a rather strong fear generalization on the explicit cognitive level in this group and a strong influence of such cognitive processes on electrodermal responses (Hamm and Weike 2005). We speculate that such interplay between cognitive and electrodermal responses may be stronger, the weaker the memory traces are, thus in the remote-test group.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 76%
“…This is supported by a number of studies that show that verbal instructions can result in the acquisition of defensive responses (Cameron et al, 2016;Costa, Bradley, & Lang, 2015;Grillon, Ameli, Woods, Merikangas, & Davis, 1991) and subjective feelings of fear and distress (Raes, De Houwer, De Schryver, Brass, & Kalisch, 2014;Soeter & Kindt, 2012), which are considered to be affective measures of fear (Hamm & Weike, 2005;Soeter & Kindt, 2012). Such results call into question whether distinctions should be made between the processes underlying learning via verbal instructions and other types of learning.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Affective learning is the acquisition of defensive responses to potentially threatening stimuli. This type of learning is proposed to take place in an automatic way and is assumed to be independent of cognitive contingency learning (Hamm & Weike, 2005;Mineka & Öhman, 2002). Cognitive contingency learning, on the other hand, refers to the purely cognitive learning of contingencies between events.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%