2018
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12728
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The ontogeny of intent‐based normative judgments

Abstract: When evaluating norm transgressions, children begin to show some sensitivity to the agent's intentionality around preschool age. However, the specific developmental trajectories of different forms of such intent‐based judgments and their cognitive underpinnings are still largely unclear. The current studies, therefore, systematically investigated the development of intent‐based normative judgments as a function of two crucial factors: (a) the type of the agent's mental state underlying a normative transgressio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
18
2
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
0
18
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…It has indeed been found that even infants hold sociomoral expectations and express intuitive preferences that take into account agents' intentions and prioritize them over actions' outcomes (Dunfield & Kuhlmeier, 2010;Hamlin, 2013;Hamlin, Ullman, Tenenbaum, Goodman, & Baker, 2013;Strid & Meristo, 2020;Woo, Steckler, Le, & Hamlin, 2017). Furthermore, we know that at least by five years of age, children consistently weigh intentions more than outcomes when they are asked to express a moral judgment in tasks that are more demanding compared to those employed with infants (Cushman, Sheketoff, Wharton, & Carey, 2013;Margoni & Surian, 2017Proft & Rakoczy, 2018; see Margoni & Surian, 2016 for a review).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has indeed been found that even infants hold sociomoral expectations and express intuitive preferences that take into account agents' intentions and prioritize them over actions' outcomes (Dunfield & Kuhlmeier, 2010;Hamlin, 2013;Hamlin, Ullman, Tenenbaum, Goodman, & Baker, 2013;Strid & Meristo, 2020;Woo, Steckler, Le, & Hamlin, 2017). Furthermore, we know that at least by five years of age, children consistently weigh intentions more than outcomes when they are asked to express a moral judgment in tasks that are more demanding compared to those employed with infants (Cushman, Sheketoff, Wharton, & Carey, 2013;Margoni & Surian, 2017Proft & Rakoczy, 2018; see Margoni & Surian, 2016 for a review).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A simpler version, perhaps based on donating stickers (cf. Proft & Rakoczy, 2019), could be useful in order to explore children's use of behavioral punishment for moral transgressions. 3…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Younger children who didn't pass a traditional ToM task attributed negative intentions to an accidental transgressor (the agent who unwittingly changed the object's location), giving more weight to the outcomes of the action than to the agent's mental states, and finding it difficult to identify a false belief when it led to a moral violation (cf. Proft & Rakoczy, 2019). However, 7-8 years olds who passed ToM tasks could attribute positive intentions to actions despite them having bad outcomes, and were more punitive with intentional transgressors.…”
Section: Investigations Of the Relationship Between Mental State Compmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It has indeed found that even infants hold sociomoral expectations that take into account agents' intentions and prioritize them over actions' outcomes (Dunfield & Kuhlmeier, 2010;Hamlin, 2013;Hamlin et al, 2013;Woo et al, 2017). Furthermore, we know that at least by five years of age, children consistently weigh intentions more than outcomes when they are asked explicitly to express a moral judgment (Cushman et al, 2013;Margoni & Surian, 2017Proft & Rakoczy, 2018;see Margoni & Surian, 2016…”
Section: Judging Accidental Harm: Due Care and Foreseeability Of Sidementioning
confidence: 99%