2004
DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.118.1.71
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Ontogeny of Serial-Order Behavior in Humans (Homo sapiens): Representation of a List.

Abstract: The authors trained 3-, 4-, 7-, and 10-year-old children and adults (Homo sapiens) on a nonverbal serial-order task to respond to 5 items in a specific order. Knowledge of each item's sequential position was then examined using pairwise and triplet tests. Adults and 7- and 10-year-olds performed at high levels on both tests, whereas 3- and 4-year-olds did not. The latency to respond to the first item of a test pair or triplet was linearly related to that item's position in the training series for the 7- and 10… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0
3

Year Published

2005
2005
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
0
7
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Although handedness was not expressed during the symbolic task, a serial order of evaluation bias may have occurred (cf. Guyla & Colombo, 2004). Expressing handedness seems to require an actual trap-tube apparatus and a stick tool.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Although handedness was not expressed during the symbolic task, a serial order of evaluation bias may have occurred (cf. Guyla & Colombo, 2004). Expressing handedness seems to require an actual trap-tube apparatus and a stick tool.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Similarly congruent distance and magnitude functions were obtained from experiments in which non-human primates and adult and juvenile humans were trained using the SCP to learn arbitrary sequences composed of photographs or geometric forms [22,23,[37][38][39]. These are shown in Figures 3c and d (Figures 3a and b).…”
Section: Distance and Magnitude Effectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although human and non-human primates are capable of sequencing both arbitrarily or meaningfully related items after a training period (D'Amato and Colombo 1988; Guyla and Colombo 2004;Swartz et al 1991Swartz et al , 2000Terrace 2001), and although monotonic sequences are easier to learn and recall than non-monotonic ones (Brannon and Terrace 2000;Ohshiba 1997;Terrace and McGonigle 1994), young human children and non-human great apes, as represented by orangutans, do not use content cues spontaneously to sequence items without some practice and training. The fact that older children (5-and 7-year-olds) use these cues readily (Terrace and McGonigle 1994), while younger children (3-and 4-year-olds) and orangutans do not, suggests that seriating items this way is a culturally learned behavior.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The cognitive task has been used in numerous experiments with pigeons, monkeys, apes, and humans (Ohshiba 1997;Subiaul et al 2004Subiaul et al , 2007Swartz et al 1991Swartz et al , 2000Terrace 1991Terrace , 2005Terrace et al 2003). Previous research into serial memory has generally used arbitrarily related items that are not inherently ordered by visual cues like size, for example (Guyla and Colombo 2004;Harris et al 2007;Subiaul et al 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%