2013
DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2013.824530
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The other-race effect: Holistic coding differences and beyond

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
78
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 81 publications
(84 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
5
78
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Yet people have difficulty discriminating between faces from ethnic and racial groups other than their own (e.g., Herrmann, Schreppel, Jäger, Ehlis, & Fallgatter, 2007;Lindsay, Jack, & Christian, 1991;Walker & Tanaka, 2003), an effect that translates into a robust memory deficit for other-race faces (Malpass & Kravitz, 1969;Meissner & Brigham, 2001;Sporer, 2001). It has been suggested that other-race faces may be processed less holistically than own-race faces (Rossion & Michel, 2011), yet support for this hypothesis varies across tasks (see Hayward, Crookes, & Rhodes, 2013, for a review). Here, we investigated the other-race effect in the complete paradigm of the composite task, which measures failures to selectively attend to one region of a face (Young, Hellawell, & Hay, 1987;Richler, Cheung, & Gauthier, 2011a).…”
Section: Taskmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet people have difficulty discriminating between faces from ethnic and racial groups other than their own (e.g., Herrmann, Schreppel, Jäger, Ehlis, & Fallgatter, 2007;Lindsay, Jack, & Christian, 1991;Walker & Tanaka, 2003), an effect that translates into a robust memory deficit for other-race faces (Malpass & Kravitz, 1969;Meissner & Brigham, 2001;Sporer, 2001). It has been suggested that other-race faces may be processed less holistically than own-race faces (Rossion & Michel, 2011), yet support for this hypothesis varies across tasks (see Hayward, Crookes, & Rhodes, 2013, for a review). Here, we investigated the other-race effect in the complete paradigm of the composite task, which measures failures to selectively attend to one region of a face (Young, Hellawell, & Hay, 1987;Richler, Cheung, & Gauthier, 2011a).…”
Section: Taskmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Free from illusion-induced interference, these individuals outperformed controls when the target and distractor halves were aligned (Le Grand et al, 2004). When tested using the original matching design, Michel and colleagues (2006) found that observers' susceptibility to the composite illusion was also greater when participants viewed composite faces of their own race (but for conflicting results obtained with the complete design see Horry, Cheong, & Brewer, 2015;Zhao, Hayward, & Bülthoff, 2014; for a review of this literature, see Hayward, Crookes, & Rhodes, 2013). Similarly, Susilo and colleagues (2009) found stronger effects using the original matching procedure when composites were constructed from own-age faces (but see Wiese, Kachel, & Schweinberger, 2013).…”
Section: Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This holistic processing hypothesis is indirectly supported by research on holistic processing in own- vs. other-race face recognition (Michel, Rossion, Han, Chung, & Caldara, 2006; Michel, Caldara, & Rossion, 2006; Mondloch et al, 2010; Rhodes, Tan, Brake, & Taylor, 1989; Tanaka, Kiefer, & Bukach, 2004). For example, studies have found that own-race faces are processed more holistically than other-race faces using a face inversion paradigm (e.g., Rhodes, Tan, Brake, & Taylor, 1989), face composite paradigm (Michel, Rossion, Han, Chung, & Caldara, 2006), and part-whole paradigm (Leder & Carbon, 2005; Michel, Caldara, & Rossion, 2006; Mondloch et al, 2010; Tanaka, Kiefer, & Bukach, 2004), although the robustness of the holistic processing effect has recently been questioned (Hayward, Crookes, & Rhodes, 2013). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%