2015
DOI: 10.1002/acp.3183
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Persistence of Erroneous Information in Memory: The Effect of Valence on the Acceptance of Corrected Information

Abstract: People often have difficulty changing previously held, but erroneous, beliefs. This finding is particularly worrisome in politics where misinformation is regularly distributed about political candidates. We examined whether initial inferences about a fictional political candidate could be corrected, and whether people's willingness to accept a correction was influenced by the valence of the information being corrected. Participants read a list of statements describing a politician running for re-election in wh… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
13
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
3
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Although false belief is reduced relative to individuals exposed to the misinformation without retraction, it remains greater than for individuals who were never exposed to the misinformation. This continued influence effect generalizes to other stimuli, including scientific journal articles (Greitemeyer, 2014), political information (Guillory & Geraci, 2013, 2015; Lewandowsky et al, 2005), and other fictional news stories (Ecker et al, 2017; Ecker, Lewandowsky, & Apai, 2011; Johnson & Seifert, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…Although false belief is reduced relative to individuals exposed to the misinformation without retraction, it remains greater than for individuals who were never exposed to the misinformation. This continued influence effect generalizes to other stimuli, including scientific journal articles (Greitemeyer, 2014), political information (Guillory & Geraci, 2013, 2015; Lewandowsky et al, 2005), and other fictional news stories (Ecker et al, 2017; Ecker, Lewandowsky, & Apai, 2011; Johnson & Seifert, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…However, there is also evidence to suggest that negative information is in general somewhat more attentiondemanding (Rozin & Royzman, 2001). For example, Guillory and Geraci (2016) found that unselected participants demonstrated a negativity bias, preferentially referring to negative information regardless of a retraction. However, the inference questions in Guillory and Geraci's study differed across scenarios in their potential to elicit references to the critical information, so methodological artifacts might have contributed to this finding; in the current study, we addressed this by matching questions across scenarios as closely as possible.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A second, independent scorer coded 40 randomly selected questionnaires (5 per condition) in order to calculate inter-rater reliability, which was high, r = .91. Following ample precedent (e.g.,Ecker & Ang, 2018;Ecker, Hogan, & Lewandowsky, 2017;Guillory & Geraci, 2016), scoring of inference questions was based on references made to the critical piece of information. Responses to open-ended inference questions were scored 1 if there was any uncontroverted reference made to the critical information, and 0 otherwise.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The continued-influence effect of misinformation refers to the consistent finding that misinformation continues to influence people’s beliefs and reasoning even after it has been corrected (Chan, Jones, Hall Jamieson, & Albarracín, 2017; Ecker, Lewandowsky, & Apai, 2011b; Ecker, Lewandowsky, Swire, & Chang, 2011a; Ecker, Lewandowsky, & Tang, 2010; Gordon, Brooks, Quadflieg, Ecker, & Lewandowsky, 2017; Guillory & Geraci, 2016; Johnson & Seifert, 1994; Rich & Zaragoza, 2016; Wilkes & Leatherbarrow, 1988; for a review, see Lewandowsky, Ecker, Seifert, Schwarz, & Cook, 2012). Misinformation can have a lasting effect on people’s reasoning, even when they demonstrably remember that the information has been corrected (Johnson & Seifert, 1994) and are given prior warnings about the persistence of misinformation (Ecker et al, 2010).…”
Section: The Continued-influence Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…With only a few exceptions (Guillory & Geraci, 2013, 2016; Rich & Zaragoza, 2016), research concerning reliance on misinformation has used open-ended questions administered in the lab (see Capella, Ophir, & Sutton, 2018, for an overview of approaches to measuring misinformation beliefs). There are several good reasons for using such questions, particularly on memory-based tasks that involve the comprehension or recall of previously studied text.…”
Section: Using Open- and Closed-ended Questions Onlinementioning
confidence: 99%