1970
DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(70)90109-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The processing of conceptual information on spatial directions from pictorial and linguistic symbols

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

2
38
1

Year Published

1978
1978
2004
2004

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
2
38
1
Order By: Relevance
“…When the meaning of the word was incongruent with the position (e.g., the word "north" at the bottom of the square), reaction time (RT) increased compared to a control condition in which the word was a nonsense syllable. Similarly, Shor (1970) found that when subjects indicated the direction of an arrow pointing up, down, left, or right, RT increased compared to a control condition when words located within the arrows were different from the direction of the arrow. Fox, Shor, and Steinman (1971) showed that interference to naming spatial direction decreased as the semantic aspects of the words ranged from incongruent names to space-related words to common words, rare words, and nonsense words.…”
mentioning
confidence: 83%
“…When the meaning of the word was incongruent with the position (e.g., the word "north" at the bottom of the square), reaction time (RT) increased compared to a control condition in which the word was a nonsense syllable. Similarly, Shor (1970) found that when subjects indicated the direction of an arrow pointing up, down, left, or right, RT increased compared to a control condition when words located within the arrows were different from the direction of the arrow. Fox, Shor, and Steinman (1971) showed that interference to naming spatial direction decreased as the semantic aspects of the words ranged from incongruent names to space-related words to common words, rare words, and nonsense words.…”
mentioning
confidence: 83%
“…Although facilitation was larger than inhibition in this study, this pattern apparently is unique to the movement stimuli used by Dyer. For example, a study by Shor (1970), described later in this section, showed substantial interference and little facilitation when the nonverbal stimulus dimension was the direction in which an arrow pointed. In general, the spatial Stroop effect seems to reflect primarily interference when the irrelevant dimension is incongruent with the relevant dimension, but the interference is less than for the Stroop color-naming task (Clark & BrowneIl, 1975;Dalrymple-Alford, 1972;Dyer, 1973Dyer, , 1974MacLeod, 1991;Shor, 1970).…”
Section: The Basic Phenomenonmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…FinaIly, Shor (1970) conducted experiments using a spatial and pictorial analog to the standard Stroop task in which the two stimulus dimensions were not integrated. The direction name UP, DOWN, LEFT, or RIGHT was embedded within an outline drawing ofan arrow pointing in one ofthe four directions.…”
Section: The Basic Phenomenonmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Stroop effect is commonly called an impairment (usually slowdown) in responding to complex stimuli containing conflicting information, in comparison with responding to neutral stimuli varying only in the attended dimension (Pieters, 1981;Shor, 1975;Stroop, 1935). Although Stroop used only color words with colored ink, the hundreds of subsequent studies have demonstrated analogous effects with other dimensions and modalities, such as visual combinations of pictureword (Lupker& Katz, 1982;Rosinki, Golinkoff, & Kukish, 1975), or word-arrow (Shor, 1970), as well as auditory combinations of word-pitch (Hamers & Lambert, 1972;Melara & Marks, 1990c).…”
Section: Gamer Interference and Effects Of Congruencementioning
confidence: 99%