2009
DOI: 10.1080/13506280902764539
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The prominence of behavioural biases in eye guidance

Abstract: When attempting to understand where people look during scene perception, researchers typically focus on the relative contributions of low-and high-level cues. Computational models of the contribution of low-level features to fixation selection, with modifications to incorporate top-down sources of information have been abundant in recent research. However, we are still some way from a model that can explain many of the complexities of eye movement behaviour. Here we show that understanding biases in how we mov… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

9
199
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 160 publications
(208 citation statements)
references
References 69 publications
(56 reference statements)
9
199
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We are not the first to point this out, and research has begun to describe systematic biases and their causes during scene viewing in laboratory conditions (Tatler & Vincent, 2009;Foulsham & Underwood, 2008;Clarke & Tatler, 2014;Tseng et al, 2008). Clarke and Tatler (2014) have recently argued that -26-choosing an accurate central baseline is particularly important when assessing the performance of different models for predicting fixations in scenes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…We are not the first to point this out, and research has begun to describe systematic biases and their causes during scene viewing in laboratory conditions (Tatler & Vincent, 2009;Foulsham & Underwood, 2008;Clarke & Tatler, 2014;Tseng et al, 2008). Clarke and Tatler (2014) have recently argued that -26-choosing an accurate central baseline is particularly important when assessing the performance of different models for predicting fixations in scenes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…If we are interested in understanding naturalistic eye movement behavior, free viewing static images is not the most representative condition (14)(15)(16)(17)(18). Understanding image-based fixation behavior is not only a question of "where?…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Accounting for the entirety of human eye movement behavior in naturalistic settings will require incorporating information about the task, high-level scene properties, and mechanistic constraints on the eye movement system (12,(15)(16)(17)(20)(21)(22). Our gold standard contains the influence of high-level (but still purely imagedependent) factors to the extent that they are consistent across observers.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite recent progress in understanding the factors that determine where an observer will eventually look in a scene, we know very little about what determines how an observer decides where he or she will specifically look next. Some evidence suggests that visual salience (Itti & Koch, 2000), occulomotor biases (Tatler & Vincent, 2009), the locations of prior fixations (Dodd, Van der Stigchel, & Hollingworth, 2009;Klein & MacInnes, 1999), semantic content (Underwood & Foulsham, 2006), and momentary task relevance (Hayhoe, Shrivastava, Mruczek, & Pelz, 2003) likely play roles, but identifying the factors that influence saccade sequencing remains an underdeveloped area of research that requires much greater scrutiny (Henderson, 2011). In this article, we consider the degree to which one's representation of a fixated object's spatio-temporal properties influences the direction in which a subsequent eye movement will travel.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite recent progress in understanding the factors that determine where an observer will eventually look in a scene, we know very little about what determines how an observer decides where he or she will specifically look next. Some evidence suggests that visual salience (Itti & Koch, 2000), occulomotor biases (Tatler & Vincent, 2009) (Henderson, 2011). In this article, we consider the degree to which one's representation of a fixated object's spatio-temporal properties influences the direction in which a subsequent eye movement will travel.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%