Abstract:According to the story model of Pennington and Hastie, jurors collect information at trial and modify it with general knowledge to create case stories. Schank and Ableson argue that human memory is organized to tell and understand stories. However, Finkel and Groscup questioned the use of manipulated, experimenter-constructed narratives to demonstrate the existence of multiple prototypical crime stories. We interviewed 76 jury eligible, death qualified citizens and asked them to imagine a first-degree murder s… Show more
“…Because of this, jurors in rape or sexual assault cases are likely to draw on their existing knowledge or cognitive schemas to interpret what happened and how blame should be apportioned (Ellison & Munro, 2010; Pennington & Hastie, 1992). In generating these accounts, research has repeatedly demonstrated that people are influenced by schemas, which encompass both crime prototypes (Finkel & Groscup, 1997; Smith, 1991; Wiener, Richmond, Seib, Rauch, & Hackney, 2002)—cognitive representations of the typical or ideal features present in a particular crime—and stereotypes—simplified evaluative cognitive representations of the people involved in the crime. It is therefore critical to understand how jurors use these schemas to interpret information and form impressions about the blameworthiness of the defendant and the complainant.…”
Jurors rely on a range of schemas when evaluating allegations of rape and sexual assault. For example, they may be influenced by the prototypicality of the alleged offense, the stereotypicality of the victim, or gender-related stereotypes. These schemas have often been conflated however, making it difficult to determine the unique impact of each on jurors' perceptions. To be able to effectively counter any schema-related misconceptions, we must first identify which beliefs are important and when. An experiment (N = 420) examined the independent effects of offense prototypicality and victim stereotypicality on mock jurors' perceptions. As expected, victim stereotypicality had a greater effect on judgments in the counter-prototypical (acquaintance) assault scenario than in the prototypical (stranger) assault scenario. When the complainant was described as being a counter-stereotypical victim in the acquaintance rape scenario, the defendant was seen as less likely to be guilty and evaluated more positively and the complainant less positively compared with when the complainant was described as being a stereotypical victim. Analysis of the qualitative data suggested a focus on different factors in reaching verdicts in the stranger and acquaintance rape scenarios. Results were interpreted as evidence that jurors "step down" through a hierarchy of schemas in their attempts to determine what happened in cases of rape and sexual assault.
“…Studies that have measured schema have used qualitative methods initially to identify schema domain and elements. Several have used traditional prototype methodology , asking respondents to picture a type of person/situation in their mind and then describe characteristics of the person/situation in detail (Skeem & Golding 2001; Weiner, Richmond, Seib, Rauch & Hackney, 2002). Thought-listing techniques, another approach to identifying cognitive structures (Cacciopo, vonHippel & Ernst 1997) ask respondents to list their thoughts about a real or hypothetical experience or anticipated event.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Conover and Feldman (1984) used this method to examine how people organize political beliefs and found support for both a schematic model of political beliefs and their strategy for studying schematic knowledge. Studies have applied cluster analysis (Lurigio & Carroll, 1985;Skeem & Golding,2001; Weiner, et al, 2002) and/or factor analytic techniques (Skeem & Golding, 2001; Conover & Feldman , 1984) to rated item data to identify and compare cognitive representations across groups of people. The evidence here suggests that groups of people with more shared and repeated experiences of a subculture (Harris, 1994), such as probation officers (Lurigio & Carroll, 1985), have more efficient coalescence in categories of schema than in a broader population with a more diffuse and limited experience base, such as jurors (Skeem & Golding, 2001).…”
Police officers frequently respond to calls involving persons with mental illnesses and in doing so, they are key gatekeepers of access to mental health treatment as well as entry into the criminal justice system. Programs such as Crisis Intervention Teams (CIT) are being implemented across the United States and elsewhere to train officers to respond more effectively and facilitate access to mental health services when appropriate. These programs would benefit from a thorough understanding of these encounters from the perspective of police officers. We take as a premise that officers develop frames of reference or “schema” for understanding and responding to these encounters that are shaped by socialization, training, and their experience as police officers. In this study, we examine police officer schema of mental/emotional disturbance (M/EDP) calls. Qualitative interviews provided the foundation to develop the Needs on the Street Interview (NOSI) to tap officer schema of four types of M/EDP scenarios. The NOSI was administered to 147 officers in Chicago and Philadelphia. Latent Class Analysis (LCA) was conducted separately for each scenario to examine groups of officers with different schema as well as predictors of schema group. For three of the four scenarios, officers were classified into a two category or schema model, for the fourth (crime reported) a three category model was supported. Schema groups tended to be differentiated by ratings of level of resistance/threat and substance use. Contrary to our expectations, CIT and law enforcement experience did not predict officer schema group. While the CIT model emphasizes de-escalation skills to reduce resistance and the need for officers to use force, CIT and other training programs may want to consider increasing content related to factors such as co-occurring substance use and managing resistance.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.