1982
DOI: 10.1016/s0022-5371(82)90670-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The relationship between monosyllables and pure tones: An investigation of phonetic symbolism

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
49
0

Year Published

1982
1982
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
3
49
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Females and males might then be drawn to gender stereotypical vowel space, leading to gender moderated vowel changes. 13 Although the authors do not mention it, there is some evidence of a sound symbolic association between high-front vowels (low-back vowels) and femininity (masculinity; Greenberg & Jenkins, 1966;Tarte, 1982;Wu, Klink, & Guo, 2013;cf. Sidhu & Pexman, 2015).…”
Section: Mechanisms For Associations Between Phonetic and Semantic Fementioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Females and males might then be drawn to gender stereotypical vowel space, leading to gender moderated vowel changes. 13 Although the authors do not mention it, there is some evidence of a sound symbolic association between high-front vowels (low-back vowels) and femininity (masculinity; Greenberg & Jenkins, 1966;Tarte, 1982;Wu, Klink, & Guo, 2013;cf. Sidhu & Pexman, 2015).…”
Section: Mechanisms For Associations Between Phonetic and Semantic Fementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Much of Walker's work focuses a core set of connotations including: light/heavy, sharp/blunt, quick/slow, bright/dark, and small/large (e.g., P. Walker & Walker, 2012). Others have focused on connotations that comprise the three factors of connotative meaning discovered by Osgood, Suci, and Tannenbaum (1957), namely, evaluation (e.g., good/bad), potency (e.g., strong/ weak), and activity (e.g., active/passive; e.g., Miron, 1961;Tarte, 1982).…”
Section: Mechanisms For Associations Between Phonetic and Semantic Fementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Other evidence, in various forms, confirms the same cross-sensory associations involving auditory pitch in people who are not regarded as being synaesthetes (i.e., the general population). For example, when typical English speaking adults draw music they are listening to, they draw lines and forms that are more angular (sharper), brighter, higher on the page, Running head: CROSS-SENSORY CORRESPONDENCES 6 smaller, and thinner, the higher in pitch and/or faster in tempo is the music (Karwoski, Odbert, & Osgood, 1942; Kussner & Leech-Wilkinson, 2013 (Boltz, 2011;Collier & Hubbard, 2001Eitan & Timmers, 2010; Marks, 1974 Marks, , 1975 Marks, , 1978Perrott, Musicant, & Schwethelm, 1980;Tarte, 1982;L. Walker, P. Walker, & Francis, 2012;P.…”
Section: The Cross-sensory Features Of Soundsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Walker & Smith, 1985;. For the three core dimensions of height (auditory pitch), brightness, and size, therefore, there is evidence confirming the Running head: CROSS-SENSORY CORRESPONDENCES 21 coherent transitivity of the associations among them (as illustrated in Figure 5A), and for the amodal and conceptual nature of the representations being linked.There is also evidence, albeit less extensive, confirming the transitivity of the cross-sensory correspondences among other sets of three feature dimensions.Specifically, equivalent evidence emerges when auditory pitch and visual surface brightness together are considered in conjunction with other feature dimensions, including pointiness (Marks, 1987;Parise & Spence, 2009;Tarte, 1982;P. Walker, 2012a;P.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%