2018
DOI: 10.1111/nyas.13714
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The removal of information from working memory

Abstract: What happens to goal‐relevant information in working memory after it is no longer needed? Here, we review evidence for a selective removal process that operates on outdated information to limit working memory load and hence facilitates the maintenance of goal‐relevant information. Removal alters the representations of irrelevant content so as to reduce access to it, thereby improving access to the remaining relevant content and also facilitating the encoding of new information. Both behavioral and neural evide… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

12
118
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 128 publications
(130 citation statements)
references
References 87 publications
12
118
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The interpretation was that the IMI had been removed from WM after the second retrocue, an operation that would include the removal of that item's synaptic trace. (For a more detailed consideration of how information might be removed from WM, see Lewis-Peacock et al, (2018).) Finally, to assess whether there were functional consequences of UMI decodability from the spTMS-evoked response, Rose et al (2016) carried out an additional experiment in which 40% of nonmatching recognition probes were the UMI (i.e., lures).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The interpretation was that the IMI had been removed from WM after the second retrocue, an operation that would include the removal of that item's synaptic trace. (For a more detailed consideration of how information might be removed from WM, see Lewis-Peacock et al, (2018).) Finally, to assess whether there were functional consequences of UMI decodability from the spTMS-evoked response, Rose et al (2016) carried out an additional experiment in which 40% of nonmatching recognition probes were the UMI (i.e., lures).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As yet there is no detailed cognitive model of the processes involved in active updating in running span. One possibility derived from the removal account of updating (Kessler & Oberauer, 2014;Lewis-Peacock, Kessler & Oberauer, 2018;Oberauer, 2018) is that all items in the target set could be actively unbound from their current positions successively. The first item would remain unbound (and thus removed) while the remaining relevant items would then be rebound to new positions allowing the new item to be incorporated into the target set.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Serial order in this model is encoded as a context code associated with each item using Hebbian learning. It was proposed that item removal is supported by an opposite process of Hebbian anti-learning involving unbinding an item from its positional context (Lewis-Peacock et al, 2018;Oberauer, 2018).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Alternatively, we could conceptualize distractor inhibition as a mechanism that ultimately aims to permanently remove irrelevant items from working memory. Such a permanent removal has been conceptualized to operate through an un-binding mechanism, that unties the present item-context bindings 39 , rather than affecting the representation of the item itself. This is in line with recent evidence, demonstrating that alpha oscillations carry information about the maintained location (i.e., the "context" in the present case) but not about location-unrelated stimulus-features (i.e., the sound's identity) 25 .…”
Section: Alpha Lateralization As a Proxy Of Target Prioritization Andmentioning
confidence: 99%