1988
DOI: 10.3758/bf03213476
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The replacement effect: Repeating some items while replacing others

Abstract: We performed three experiments on recognition learning that tested for the existence of a replacement effect (i.e., the benefit accruing to nonrecognized items, or targets, when recognized items are replaced in the next study trial). A reverse Rock substitution procedure was used, and the replacement effect occurred in all three experiments. The results were interpreted in terms of a distributed memory model, the matched-filter model of Anderson (1973), but several modifications were necessary. The original ve… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
51
0

Year Published

1988
1988
2007
2007

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(53 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
2
51
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Although the results from Bostrom and Bower, Rock (1957), and Murdock and Babick (1961) paralleled those of the present study, more recently Murdock and Lamon (1988) have presented evidence that made a different set of conclusions possible. In a recognition learning paradigm in which recognized words on one trial were replaced on the next trial, the repetition of unrecognized words resulted in greater recognition (relative to recognition of replacement words) than did a control condition in which no replacement took place.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 60%
“…Although the results from Bostrom and Bower, Rock (1957), and Murdock and Babick (1961) paralleled those of the present study, more recently Murdock and Lamon (1988) have presented evidence that made a different set of conclusions possible. In a recognition learning paradigm in which recognized words on one trial were replaced on the next trial, the repetition of unrecognized words resulted in greater recognition (relative to recognition of replacement words) than did a control condition in which no replacement took place.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 60%
“…In fitting our data, NEMO consistently chose a smaller standard deviation for the memorial noise of the more recent stimulus (see Table 2). Borrowing from the literature on global-matching models of recognition memory (Murdock & Kahana, 1993b;Murdock & Lamon, 1988), we also allow NEMO to differentially weight the contribution of s 1 and s 2 in the summed-similarity calculation (this is done via the model's parameter). This increased weighting of recently experienced items may be viewed either as a decay process or as a mechanism of strategic control.…”
Section: Fitting Nemo To Mnemometric Functionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…TODAM, CHARM, and the Matrix model provide examples of models in which simply adding more copies of each trace to memory may not improve memory: Both the mean and standard deviation of the retrieved signal rise together in such a way that performance does not change (Murdock & Lamon 1988;Shiffrin et ai 1990). At least two approaches have been used to solve this problem.…”
Section: Differences and Similaritiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At least two approaches have been used to solve this problem. Hintzman (1986Hintzman ( , 1988 and Murdock & Lamon (1988;see also Murdock 1989) have proposed a probabilistic encoding assumption: Each feature of an item is encoded (stored) with a probability p that rises with presentation time. If not stored it is given a neutral value (or in a variant discussed by Shiffrin et al 1990, replaced by random value).…”
Section: Differences and Similaritiesmentioning
confidence: 99%