2010
DOI: 10.1348/026151009x424565
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The role of gaze direction and mutual exclusivity in guiding 24‐month‐olds' word mappings

Abstract: In these studies, we examined how a default assumption about word meaning, the mutual exclusivity assumption and an intentional cue, gaze direction, interacted to guide 24-month-olds' object-word mappings. In Expt 1, when the experimenter's gaze was consistent with the mutual exclusivity assumption, novel word mappings were facilitated. When the experimenter's eye-gaze was in conflict with the mutual exclusivity cue, children demonstrated a tendency to rely on the mutual exclusivity assumption rather than foll… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
18
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
2
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Ostensive naming may facilitate word learning due to social pragmatic cues. A rich literature demonstrates that social pragmatic cues, such as pointing and eye-gaze, are highly beneficial in early word learning (e.g., Akhtar and Tomasello, 1996; Moore et al, 1999; Graham et al, 2010; Grassmann and Tomasello, 2010). Horst and Samuelson (2008) used pointing as a social pragmatic cue during ostensive naming, but no cues during follow-in labeling.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Ostensive naming may facilitate word learning due to social pragmatic cues. A rich literature demonstrates that social pragmatic cues, such as pointing and eye-gaze, are highly beneficial in early word learning (e.g., Akhtar and Tomasello, 1996; Moore et al, 1999; Graham et al, 2010; Grassmann and Tomasello, 2010). Horst and Samuelson (2008) used pointing as a social pragmatic cue during ostensive naming, but no cues during follow-in labeling.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, toddlers tend to ignore gaze when it is in conflict with the child's knowledge of lexical conventions or when a speaker looks at a familiar object and uses a novel word (Graham, Nilsen, Collins, & Olineck, 2010;Jaswal, 2010;Jaswal & Hansen, 2006).…”
Section: Pragmatic Information Children Use In Word Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, when a speaker does not speak a certain language or has a history of unconventional language use, children do not seem to exclude any of the available objects as potential referents of the speaker's utterance (Diesendruck, 2005;Diesendruck, Carmel, & Markson, 2010;Diesendruck & Markson, 2001;Grassmann, Schreiner, & Tomasello, 2011;Sobel, Sedivy, Buchanan, & Hennessy, 2011). Forth, exclusion is facilitated when the speaker's gaze, gestures, and/or language provides converging evidence (Graham et al, 2010;Saylor, Sabbagh, & Baldwin, 2002).…”
Section: Contrast and Conventionalitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Like imitation, labelling occurs within a social context. Parental object labelling is frequent and predictable during early development such that by 2 years of age, children reliably use social cues (eye‐gaze) conveyed by a social agent to learn object‐label pairings (Graham, Nilsen, Collins, & Olineck, ). These types of social learning environments also support toddlers’ ability to use a provided label to transfer learning from pictures to real‐world objects (Geraghty, Waxman, & Gelman, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%