1983
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000900005134
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The role of input frequency in lexical acquisition

Abstract: The influence of frequency of occurrence in input upon early lexical acquisition was examined within an experimental paradigm. Twelve children (1; 0·21 to 1; 3·15) were presented with 16 contrived lexical concepts, each involving a nonsense word and four referents, over ten experimental sessions. Within each concept two exemplars were presented frequently and two were presented infrequently. Overall the children named more frequently presented exemplars than infrequently presented exemplars. However, when the … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
60
0
1

Year Published

1986
1986
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 107 publications
(62 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
1
60
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In other studies involving large sets of target words, learning has also been limited. For example, Schwartz and Terrell (1983) found toddlers to learn, on average, only 5 or 6 of 16 novel words presented multiple times over the course of 10 weeks. Here 20 words were presented and, judging by their performance in session 1, the children were unfamiliar with the majority of them.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In other studies involving large sets of target words, learning has also been limited. For example, Schwartz and Terrell (1983) found toddlers to learn, on average, only 5 or 6 of 16 novel words presented multiple times over the course of 10 weeks. Here 20 words were presented and, judging by their performance in session 1, the children were unfamiliar with the majority of them.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Learning words from exposures distributed over time is generally more effective than learning from exposures massed at a single point in time, even when the overall number of exposures is held constant (Childers & Tomasello, 2002; Riches, Tomasello, & ContiRamsden, 2005;Schwartz & Terrell). The benefits of distributed exposure hold for both initial phases of learning and for later retention (Bahrick & Phelps, 1987).…”
Section: Frequency Of Exposurementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Word frequency exerts a strong influence on the likelihood of an error in adults; low-frequency words are more subject to error than high-frequency words (Dell, 1990;Stemberger & MacWhinney, 1985). In the case of the novice learner, the strength of a word will depend on its experienced frequency, that is, on the actual number of times children have the opportunity to hear and say the word (Schwartz & Terrell, 1983). Early in development, when children first begin to name objects, all of their words will be of low frequency.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Even stronger evidence for the generality of the noun advantage comes from studies by Schwartz, Camarata, and Leonard in which children were presented with novel words, either as nouns or as verbs, and then tested for the production of these words. Even when stress, frequency, phonological makeup, and word order are equated, children are more likely to produce words experienced as nouns than as verbs (Camarata & Leonard, 1986;Camarata & Schwartz, 1985;Schwartz & Terrell, 1983). Thus, it appears that the reasons for the early noun advantage are conceptual or semantic factors.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%