2022
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119533
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The role of motion in the neural representation of social interactions in the posterior temporal cortex

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

2
10
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
2
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Accumulating evidence ( Pitcher et al, 2014 ; Walbrin and Koldewyn, 2019 ; Landsiedel et al, 2022 ) suggests that dynamic visual aspects of social perception (e.g., face, hand and body movements across the visual field) cannot be easily accommodated within the classic dual-stream model for visual perception ( Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982 ). Accordingly, resting on both anatomical and functional evidence in humans and non-human primates, Pitcher and Ungerleider (2021) proposed the existence of a third visual processing pathway ( Figure 1B ) that projects on the lateral cortical surface from the early visual cortex into the mid-posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) via motion-selective occipito-temporal areas (V5/hMT).…”
Section: Multimodal Processing In Face-to-face Interactions: a Possib...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Accumulating evidence ( Pitcher et al, 2014 ; Walbrin and Koldewyn, 2019 ; Landsiedel et al, 2022 ) suggests that dynamic visual aspects of social perception (e.g., face, hand and body movements across the visual field) cannot be easily accommodated within the classic dual-stream model for visual perception ( Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982 ). Accordingly, resting on both anatomical and functional evidence in humans and non-human primates, Pitcher and Ungerleider (2021) proposed the existence of a third visual processing pathway ( Figure 1B ) that projects on the lateral cortical surface from the early visual cortex into the mid-posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) via motion-selective occipito-temporal areas (V5/hMT).…”
Section: Multimodal Processing In Face-to-face Interactions: a Possib...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, the posterior superior temporal sulcus social interaction region (SI-pSTS) has been found to play a key role in the representation of visually perceived dynamic social interactions. Across a range of stimuli that vary in the strength of relevant social cues (e.g., point-light displays, animated shapes, videos of dyads), the SI-pSTS responds about twice as strongly to interacting dyads compared to two independently acting individuals (Isik et al, 2017; Walbrin et al, 2018; Walbrin & Koldewyn, 2019) and shows this selectivity even in naturalistic videos (Landsiedel et al, 2022; Masson & Isik, 2021). Further, multivariate decoding analyses have found that the right SI-pSTS not only discriminates between interactors and non-interactors, but also appears to represent the type and emotional content of interactions (e.g., competing/cooperating; Isik et al, 2017; Walbrin et al, 2018; Walbrin & Koldewyn, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The dominant mode within social neuroscience has been to seek out specialised neural subsystems dedicated to processing social (as opposed to more general kinds of) information (Apperly et al, 2005; Happé et al, 2017; Saxe & Powell, 2006; Spunt & Adolphs, 2017). This approach has uncovered evidence for the existence of category-sensitive cortex; regions that preferentially activate during the perception of certain social stimuli, such as faces (Kanwisher & Yovel, 2006), bodies (Downing & Kanwisher, 2010), and dyadic social interactions (Landsiedel et al, 2022). It has been argued that more complex inferential processes such as mental state attribution, or Theory of Mind, also engage highly specialised social brain areas (Apperly et al, 2005; Brüne & Brüne-Cohrs, 2006; Dodell-Feder et al, 2011; Gweon et al, 2012; Jacoby et al, 2016; Jenkins et al, 2014; Koster-Hale & Saxe, 2013; Richardson & Saxe, 2020; Ross & Olson, 2010; Saxe & Baron-Cohen, 2006; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003b; Saxe & Wexler, 2005; Scholz et al, 2009; Simmons et al, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%