1989
DOI: 10.1037/h0084218
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The role of structure in infant visual pattern perception.

Abstract: The research of the authors and others on the role of structure in infant visual pattern perception is reviewed. Two new experiments are reported on preference for symmetry in infants. K is shown that infants reliably prefer patterns with multiple axes of bilateral symmetry relative to asymmetrical patterns. Also, the results demonstrate that vertically oriented single axis bilateral symmetry is more salient than horizontally oriented symmetry in infants as it is in adults. It is argued that sensitivity to var… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
25
0
1

Year Published

1993
1993
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 72 publications
0
25
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Younger and Gotlieb ( 1988) also reported equivalent encoding rates for "good" and "poor" gestalts in 3-montholds but observed higher looking times for "poor" over "good" gestalts in infants of the same age range (although this trend was reversed in 5-to 7-month-olds). Furthermore, Humphrey and Humphrey (1989) observed both higher looking times and more rapid encoding rates for "good" over "poor" gestalts in 4-month-olds. Differences in stimulus metrics used to define "goodness" across the studies (Dodwell & Caelli, 1985;Garner, 1974;Posner, Goldsmith, & Welton, 1967) or familiarization procedures (e.g., number and duration of trials) or both may be responsible for some of the inconsistencies in outcomes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Younger and Gotlieb ( 1988) also reported equivalent encoding rates for "good" and "poor" gestalts in 3-montholds but observed higher looking times for "poor" over "good" gestalts in infants of the same age range (although this trend was reversed in 5-to 7-month-olds). Furthermore, Humphrey and Humphrey (1989) observed both higher looking times and more rapid encoding rates for "good" over "poor" gestalts in 4-month-olds. Differences in stimulus metrics used to define "goodness" across the studies (Dodwell & Caelli, 1985;Garner, 1974;Posner, Goldsmith, & Welton, 1967) or familiarization procedures (e.g., number and duration of trials) or both may be responsible for some of the inconsistencies in outcomes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…suit in greater initial looking times during the first part of the familiarization phase (Bornstein, 1978;Humphrey & Humphrey, 1989). Second, on the assumption that "good" gestalts are processed (i.e., encoded) more efficiently than "poor" gestalts, one would expect that the rate of habituation would be faster for "good" gestalts, relative to "poor" gestalts and to distortions of both "good" and "poor" gestalts (Humphrey et a\., 1986).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Known symmetry-loving animals include finches [16], bees [17], pigeons [18], starlings [19], swordtail fish [20], and even newborn chicks [21]. There is also evidence that preference for symmetry is present already in human infants [22,23]. This large body of research strongly suggests that a symmetry-sensitive visual system is an adaptive phenotype, promoted by natural selection.…”
Section: The Importance Of Symmetrymentioning
confidence: 98%