2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The self-regulating brain: Cortical-subcortical feedback and the development of intelligent action

Abstract: To speak of cognitive regulation versus emotion regulation may be misleading. However, some forms of regulation are carried out by executive processes, subject to voluntary control, while others are carried out by "automatic" processes that are far more primitive. Both sets of processes are in constant interaction, and that interaction gives rise to a stream of activity that is both cognitive and emotional. Studying the brain helps us understand these reciprocal regulatory influences in some detail. Cortical a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
121
0
7

Year Published

2009
2009
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 149 publications
(131 citation statements)
references
References 72 publications
3
121
0
7
Order By: Relevance
“…However, young children show larger conflictrelated amplitude effects compared to adults in later latencies, from about 600 to 800 ms post-target in anterior mid-frontal leads (Rueda, Posner et al, 2004). From about 6 to 8 years of age, the conflict-related amplitude effects are observed in more adult-like latencies, and the size of the effect appears to decrease with age (Jonkman, 2006;Lewis & Todd, 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…However, young children show larger conflictrelated amplitude effects compared to adults in later latencies, from about 600 to 800 ms post-target in anterior mid-frontal leads (Rueda, Posner et al, 2004). From about 6 to 8 years of age, the conflict-related amplitude effects are observed in more adult-like latencies, and the size of the effect appears to decrease with age (Jonkman, 2006;Lewis & Todd, 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Drawing on Gross' influential process model [2], we divide emotion regulation strategies into those that modulate emotional responses either before or after an emotionally salient event ( Figure 1, x axis). However, distinct from dual-process models that draw a line between conscious and unconscious processes, or propose distinct neural pathways for each [8,32], we draw on developmental research to incorporate the notion of multiple levels of control processes [47]. Drawing on Eisenberg's notion of levels of reactive and effortful control, we suggest that these range from reflexive strategies to more cognitively 'effortful' ones ( Figure 1, y axis).…”
Section: Box 1 Extended Model Of Emotion Regulationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This pattern does not necessarily illustrate age-related differences in the degree of affective bias or for the capacity for suppression of biases, but simply a change in what is more motivationally important at a given stage of life. However, it is important to note that, beyond normative developmental trends, individual differences in specific biases may become more entrenched with age [47].…”
Section: Affective Tuning Can Shift With Age and Contextmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Taken together, these primary DS concepts of variability and multiple interacting time scales enable a more precise modeling of flexibility. Borrowing from the pioneering modeling of Lewis and colleagues (Lewis, 1997(Lewis, , 2000(Lewis, , 2005Lewis & Ferrari, 2001;Lewis & Liu, 2011;Lewis & Todd, 2007), we propose our model of flexibility at three time scales.…”
Section: Dynamic Systemsmentioning
confidence: 99%