2010
DOI: 10.1126/science.1190792
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Social Sense: Susceptibility to Others’ Beliefs in Human Infants and Adults

Abstract: Human social interactions crucially depend on the ability to represent other agents' beliefs even when these contradict our own beliefs, leading to the potentially complex problem of simultaneously holding two conflicting representations in mind. Here, we show that adults and 7-month-olds automatically encode others' beliefs, and that, surprisingly, others' beliefs have similar effects as the participants' own beliefs. In a visual object detection task, participants' beliefs and the beliefs of an agent (whose … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

36
704
3
8

Year Published

2014
2014
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 722 publications
(751 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
36
704
3
8
Order By: Relevance
“…First, they confirm prior findings that by the second year of life, infants can attribute false beliefs to agents (e.g., Buttelmann et al, 2009;Kovács et al, 2010;Luo, 2011;Onishi & Baillargeon, 2005;Scott et al, 2015;Senju et al, 2011;Träuble et al, 2010). Second, these results extend these findings to a novel belief-based response: infants can reason not only about which location a mistaken agent will act on, but also about how her false belief will affect her subsequent emotional displays.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…First, they confirm prior findings that by the second year of life, infants can attribute false beliefs to agents (e.g., Buttelmann et al, 2009;Kovács et al, 2010;Luo, 2011;Onishi & Baillargeon, 2005;Scott et al, 2015;Senju et al, 2011;Träuble et al, 2010). Second, these results extend these findings to a novel belief-based response: infants can reason not only about which location a mistaken agent will act on, but also about how her false belief will affect her subsequent emotional displays.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…This growing body of evidence casts doubt on the notion that infants' successful performance in falsebelief tasks is the result of primitive abilities that apply to only a limited range of situations and behaviors. Such findings instead support recent mentalistic accounts, which propose that infants make sense of agents' behavior by reasoning about their motivational, epistemic, and counterfactual states (e.g., Baillargeon, Scott, & Bian, 2016;Baillargeon et al, 2010;Barrett et al, 2013;Buttelmann et al, 2009;Carruthers, 2013;Kovács et al, 2010;Luo, 2011;Scott et al, 2010;Southgate et al, 2007;Surian et al, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 65%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In typical development implicit mentalizing develops during the first year of life (e.g., Kovács et al 2010) and is seen as a precursor to explicit mentalizing, i.e., giving the correct reasoning for a person's (false) belief (Clements et al 2000;Low and Perner 2012;Thoermer et al 2012). Young infants, for example, track the beliefs of others (Kovács et al 2010;Onishi and Baillargeon 2005) without necessarily being able to make correct explicit belief inferences (Ruffman et al 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Young infants, for example, track the beliefs of others (Kovács et al 2010;Onishi and Baillargeon 2005) without necessarily being able to make correct explicit belief inferences (Ruffman et al 2001). During adulthood, implicit and explicit mentalizing processes seem to coexist mediating distinct features of social cognition.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%