Does phenomenal experience of a stimulus reflect the qualities of its processing? Reicher (1969) has shown that words are processed faster than nonwords. Specifically, he presented four-letter words, four-letter nonwords, and single letters very briefly. He then presented two letters and asked participants which of the two letters had appeared before. Participants showed higher performance for words than for nonwords and single letters, as reflected by higher accuracy and faster reaction times. In this article, we are interested in the performance difference between words and nonwords, an effect that is well established (see, e.g., Prinzmetal & Silvers, 1994); we are not interested in the performance difference between words and single letters. A word-superiority effect on letter discrimination indicates faster processing of words than of nonwords. In this article, we examine whether this faster processing of words may translate into a phenomenal experience of longer duration, higher figure-ground contrast, and larger size.There is much evidence that more-familiar stimuli are judged to be presented longer than less-familiar stimuli. For example, Warm and McCray (1969; see Allan, 1979, for further findings) have found that frequently used words presented for 1 sec were judged as shown longer than infrequently used words shown for the same duration. Witherspoon and Allan (1985) found that participants judged more familiar stimuli to be presented longer than unfamiliar stimuli. These findings are relevant to the studies presented here because one basis of familiarity is perceptual fluency (e.g., Whittlesea, 1993;Whittlesea, Jacoby, & Girard, 1990), which is the ease with which incoming information can be processed (for a detailed discussion, see Reber, Wurtz, & Zimmermann, 2004;Winkielman, Schwarz, Fazendeiro, & Reber, 2003).There is abundant evidence for the impact of processing fluency on several kinds of judgments, such as those of recognition (Jacoby & Dallas, 1981;Whittlesea, 1993), affective preference (Reber, Winkielman, & Schwarz, 1998;Whittlesea, 1993), fame (Jacoby, Kelley, Brown, & Jasechko, 1989), truth (Begg, Anas, & Farinacci, 1992;Reber & Schwarz, 1999), ease of performance (Kelley & Jacoby, 1996), normative word frequency (Toth & Daniels, 2002), stimulus duration (Masson & Caldwell, 1998;Witherspoon & Allan, 1985), and stimulus clarity (Goldinger, Kleider, & Shelley, 1999;Jacoby, Allan, Collins, & Larwill, 1988;Whittlesea et al., 1990). Most of these studies used stimulus repetition to manipulate processing fluency, with some exceptions. In a study by Whittlesea (1993, Experiment 6), for example, people had to judge whether a target word that had been shown before was presented for a short duration (67 msec) or for a long duration (134 msec). Before the judgment, a list of words was presented briefly, including the target word that appeared for the long duration in half of the trials and for the short duration in the other half of the trials.
1105Copyright 2004 Psychonomic Society, Inc.This research...